Thulean

From Linguifex
Jump to navigation Jump to search


Introduction

Description

The Thulean language (endonym: Minegkatsi) has an agglutinative and polysynthetic morphology. Its morphosyntactic alignment is split intransitive with the fluid-S subtype. It is left-branching and double-marking. Thulean is a language isolate and has not been proven to belong to any established language family, although speculative suggestions of links between it and Uralic or Uralo-Siberian have been made in the past. It is spoken by the Selhat or Thuleans and has a total number of speakers exceeding 30 million.

The Thurse

Thurse is the collective name in English of the European pygmy phenotype. Thurse males and females are similar in stature and have an average height of between 135 to 140 cm. They are pale-skinned with dark stripes and have straight or wavy hair. Red hair is more common among the Thurse than any other phenotypical group. High androgyny among Thurse males and high neoteny generally are also notable traits. The Thurse belong to several ethnic groups, the largest of which is the Selhat or Thuleans who occupy Thule (endonym: Telku) the northernmost of the British Isles. The Thurse phenotype is believed to have diverged from other Palaeolithic populations in Europe approximately 10,000 BP, originating in the Hercynian forest zone of central Europe.

Influences

The phonology is inspired by Finnish and Quenya phonaesthetics but twisted towards my sensibilities. I just love lateral obstruents, you know? And I'm not excessively fond of /f/. The morphology is heavily influenced by Yupik and Inuktitut.

-->


Phonology

Orthography

Thulean is written in the Latin script. It is phonemic but does show the effects of sandhi and consonant gradation. The current alphabet was adopted in 1898 and underwent its last revision in 1965. The orthography is listed in the tables below:


1) Consonants

Labial Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
Central Lateral Plain Labialised
Sonorant Nasal m n nj g gw
Liquid r l lj
Stop Fortis pp tt ttl ttj kk kkw
Lenis p t tl tj k kw x
Fricative Fortis ss ssl ssj qq qqw
Lenis s sl sj q qw
Approximant v j h w


2) Vowels

Front Back
Monophthong High i u
Low e a
Diphthong ai au


Note that the glottal stop is not written word-initially. The geminate palatal and labialised velar nasals are written "nnj" and "ggw" respectively. The geminate palatal liquid is written "llj".

Consonants

Thulean has 35 consonant phonemes which according to WALS is a large inventory. These are listed in the table below:


Labial Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
Central Lateral Plain Labialised
Sonorant Nasal /m/ /n/ /ɲ/ /ŋ/ w/
Liquid /r/ /l/ /ʎ/
Stop Fortis /pp/ /tt/ /ttɬ/ /ttʃ/ /kk/ /kkw/
Lenis /p/ /t/ /tɬ/ /tʃ/ /k/ /kw/ /ʔ/
Fricative Fortis /ss/ /ɬɬ/ /ʃʃ/ /xx/ /xxw/
Lenis /s/ /ɬ/ /ʃ/ /x/ /xw/
Approximant /v/ /j/ /h/ /w/

Vowels

Thulean has 4 vowel phonemes which according to WALS is a small inventory. There are also 2 diphthongs. The consonant to vowel ratio is 8.75 which according to WALS is high. The vowels are listed in the table below:


Front Back
Monophthong High /i/ /u/
Low /ɛ/ /ɑ/
Diphthong /ai/ /au/

Allophony

1) Fortis obstruents are realised as their corresponding lenis obstruents in intervocalic position.

2) /p, t, ʧ, k, kʷ, s, ʃ, x, xʷ/ are voiced in intervocalic position.

3) /tɬ/ is realised as [dð] in intervocalic position.

4) /ɬ/ is realised as [θ] in onset position following a coda consonant of /n, r/.

5) /ɬ/ is realised as [ð] in intervocalic position.

6) /x/ is realised as [ʃ] in coda position.

7) The geminate clusters /mm, nn, ɲɲ, ŋŋ, ŋŋʷ, rr, ll, ʎʎ/ realised as [mb, nd, ɲʤ, ŋg, ŋgʷ, rd, ld, ʎʤ].

8) /ɑ/ is realised as [ɔ] before a liquid coda.

Prosody

Stress

Thulean has stress accent. Primary stress falls within the root. Open syllables without a diphthong are light; open syllables with a diphthong or closed syllables are heavy. Fortis obstruents close the syllable preceding them. If the first syllable of a root is heavy then the primary stress falls there otherwise it falls upon the second syllable of the root. In compound nouns, the primary stress falls within the second root.The stress placement within a root is fixed.

Intonation

Secondary stress falls upon every alternate syllable after the primary stress. This gives Thulean a broadly iambic rhythm. Thulean does not have phonemic tone.

Phonotactics

1) The syllable template in Thulean is CV(C).

2) Permitted syllable codas:

a) Morpheme-medial only: /ɲ, ŋ, r, l, ʎ, p, s/

b) Morpheme-medial and morpheme-final: /m, n, t, k, x/

3) Fortis obstruents may not occur in word-initial position.

4) Fortis plosives may not occur in the onset of closed syllables.

5) Consonant clusters may have no more than two segments.

6) Consonant clusters may only occur across syllable boundaries.

7) But suffixes may begin with a consonant cluster.

8) Permitted consonant clusters as per table below:

m n ɲ ŋ ŋʷ r l ʎ p t ʧ k ʔ s ɬ ʃ x v j h w
m mm mp mv
n nn nt ntɬ ns
ɲ ɲɲ ɲʧ ɲʃ
ŋ ŋŋ ŋŋʷ ŋk ŋkʷ ŋʔ ŋx ŋxʷ ŋh
r rr rp rt rtɬ rk rkʷ rs rx rxʷ rv rj rh rw
l ll lp lt lk lkʷ ls lx lxʷ lv lh lw
ʎ ʎʎ ʎʧ ʎʃ
p ps px
t ts tx
t ks kx
s sp st stɬ sk skʷ
x xp xt xtɬ xk xkʷ

9) High vowels may not occur before a liquid coda.

10) /i/ may not occur after an onset of /j/.

11) /u/ may not occur after an onset of /w/.

12) Diphthongs may not occur in a closed syllable.

13) Diphthongs may not precede a fortis obstruent.

14) Diphthongs may not precede /j, w/.

Morphophonology

Consonant Gradation

1) Consonant gradation is word-internal lenition that effects the following:

a) Fortis obstruents

b) Lenis plosives following a sonorant coda or a vowel

2) Consonant gradation is triggered by the closing of a syllable which begins with the above classes of obstruents.

3) The sequences of consonant gradation are as per the tables below:


a)Fortis obstruents, and lenis plosives following a liquid:

GRADE I GRADE II GRADE III
pp p v
tt t r
ttɬ l
ʧ j
kk k h
kkʷ w
ss s
ɬɬ ɬ
ʃʃ ʃ
xx x
xxʷ


b) Lenis plosives following a nasal:

GRADE I GRADE II
mp mb
nt nd
ntɬ ld
ɲʧ ɲʤ
ŋk ŋg
ŋkʷ ŋgʷ


4) Consonant gradation occurs after metathesis from sandhi but before sandhi proper.

Sandhi

1) Sandhi is the term given to interactions between consonants at morpheme boundaries.

2) If a triconsonantal cluster should result from suffixation then an epenthetic /E/ is inserted after the first segment of the triconsonantal cluster.

3) All other sandhi interactions are resolved as per the table below:

m n ɲ ŋ ŋʷ r l ʎ p t ʧ k ʔ s ɬ ʃ x v j h w
m mb nd ɲʤ ŋg ŋgʷ rd ld ʎʤ mp nt ntɬ ɲʧ ŋk ŋkʷ ns ɲʃ ŋx ŋxʷ mv ɲ ŋh ŋʷ
n
t nt ɲʧ nt ŋkʷ rt ʎʧ pp tt ttɬ kk kkʷ tt ts ʧ tx kkʷ ʧ tx
k ŋk ŋk rk lk kk ks kx kx
x ŋx ŋx ŋxʷ rx lx ʎʃ ʃp ʃt ʃtɬ ʃʧ ʃk ʃkʷ ss ɬɬ ʃʃ xx xxʷ ʃ xx


4) If owing to suffixation a high vowel should precede a liquid coda then it is lowered to its corresponding low vowel.

Morphology

General Notes

Thulean morphology has the following constituents:

1) Roots: these are divided into nominal and verbal roots. The latter are divided into transitive and intransitive categories.

2) Postbases: these are derivational or adjunctival suffixes which directly follow the root. They are scope-ordered.

3) Inflectional affixes: these bear functions such as case, number, possession, agent, patient, tense amongst others. The usual bread and butter, nuts and bolts stuff that inflection does.

4) Particles: Conjunctions, interjections and other miscellany that do not fit into the above categories.

Nominal Morphology

Nominal Structure

1) Noun template:

case prefix + nominal or verbal root + postbase(s) + number suffix + possessive suffix or indefinite suffix + demonstrative suffix

2) A minimally inflected noun has a case prefix and a number suffix.

Cases

Nominal Case Declension
Case Prefix Function Examples
Absolutive Ø-
  • Marks the nominal citation form
  • Marks the O argument of a transitive verb
  • Marks the stative S argument of an intransitive verb
  • Marks the recipient of a ditransitive verb
Ergative a-
  • Marks the A argument of a transitive verb
  • Marks the active S argument of an intransitive verb
  • Marks the possessor of a possessive noun phrase
Instrumental ki-
  • Indicates the use of an instrument or tool
  • Marks the the focus of an intransitive verb of perception, cognition or affection of stative S arguments
  • Marks direct causal arguments
  • Marks the theme of a ditransitive verb
Locative jet-
  • Indicates location or place
  • Indicates time, event, or occasion
  • Marks the locative comitative
  • Marks the verbal infinitive in periphrastic constructions
Allative nu-
  • Indicates direction, goal, or destination
  • Marks the focus of an intransitive verb of perception, cognition or affection of active S arguments
  • Encodes spatial or temporal relations in conjunction with terminative markers (e.g. up to, until, as far as)
  • Marks the beneficiary of an action
Ablative pik-
  • Indicates motion away from
  • Indicates origin
  • Indicates distance from a reference point
  • Indicates time since an event occurred
  • Encodes partitive relations (part of, made from, some of)
  • Marks the focus of intransitive malefactive/adversarial verbs
  • Encodes aversive case relations (for fear of, lest, in case of)
Perlative me-
  • Indicates motion through, across or along
  • Indicates path, means or mode of transport or transmission
  • Indicates duration
  • Marks the instrumental or collaborative comitative
  • Marks indirect causal arguments
Equative sin-
  • Indicates similarity in manner, likeness or composition
  • Encodes comparative marking (as, than)
  • Serves as a topicalizer (e.g. regarding, concerning, about, as for)


Number

1) For the purposes of number, Thulean nouns are divided into count nouns and mass nouns.

2) For count nouns there are two systems of number:

a) singular-plural

b) collective-singulative

3) The singular-plural system works as it does in most European languages, the singular form of the noun is default and marks a single instance of that noun. The plural form marks multiple instances of that noun. Eg:

SG: kattu = cat

PL: katut = cats

4) With the collective-singulative system the collective form of the noun is default and marks multiple instances of that noun. The singulative form marks a single instance of that noun. Collective nouns usually indicate entities that are found in groups. Eg:

COL: makku = pigs

SGV: makuq = pig

5) Mass nouns are considered to be pluralia tanta and thus use the collective-singulative system. The collective form indicates a lump or mass and the singulative form indicates a part of that lump or mass. Eg:

COL: ninta = water

SGV: ninnuq = a drop or sip of water

6) Number suffixes:

a) After vowel:

PL: -t

SGV: -q

b) After consonant or before suffix:

PL: -ta

SGV: -qe

Possession

1) Pronominal Possession:

This is indicated by the following set of suffixes:

1SG: -nne

2SG: -gke

3SG ANIM: -nte

3SG INAN: -mme

4SG ANIM: -lle

4SG INAN: -nse

1PL EXCL: -gka

1PL INCL: -ppa

2PL: -kka

3PL ANIM: -tta

3PL INAN: -mma

4PL ANIM: -lka

4PL INAN: -ksa

2) Nominal Possession:

This is indicated by the following construction:

NOM + possessor possessum + pronominal possessive suffix eg:

akimet katunte = the woman's cat

The Indefinite

1) The indefinite suffix -li imparts the meaning of a, a certain, some, any.

2) Used with the ablative case has the meaning any of, any one of. Eg:

pikselhat telli = any one of the people

pikselhat talki = any of the people

3) When used with 3rd and 4th person pronouns creates indefinite pronouns. Eg:

telli = someone, anyone (3SG)

salki = some people, any people (4PL)

Demonstratives

1) Thulean has four demonstrative suffixes which encode the following distances:

a) The proximal citerior which marks a person or object near the speaker.

b) The distal citerior which marks a person or object near the addressee.

c) The proximal ulterior which marks a person or object away from both speaker and addressee but within visual range.

d) The distal ulterior which marks a person or object away from both speaker and addressee but outside visual range.

2)The demonstrative suffixes are as follows:

PROX CIT: -tsi

DIST CIT: -psu

PROX ULT: -tje

DIST ULT: -nja

3) By attaching the demonstrative suffixes to personal pronouns a set of demonstrative pronouns can be created. Eg:

minetsi = this thing

tukenja = yon people out of sight

Gender

1) Thulean has two genders, animate and inanimate.

2) The animate gender contains nouns referring to people, animals and dynamic physical phenomena such as fire or wind.

3) The inanimate gender contains the residuum.

4) Nouns are not overtly marked for gender but they govern the appropriate pronouns, possessive suffixes and verbal pronominal markers. Eg:

paqanti kattu = the pretty cat

helkammu ninta = the hot water


Verbal Morphology

Verbal Structure

Syntax

Constituent order

Noun phrase

Verb phrase

Sentence phrase

Dependent clauses

Example texts

Other resources

Thulean Lexicon

ahax = fire

anja = copper, small change

arkat = silver, money, cash

ausa = gold

esel = leaves

hanta = to give

helka = to be hot

isan = iron

janaq = man

kattu = cat

kimet = woman

kwetsan = feathers

-limut = should, must, ought

-mak = can, able to

makku = pigs

ninta = water

palka = to go

palla = wolves

pattu = hare

pattuhalla = donkey

penikken = pencil

qaneq = willows

qentu = to see

qwenestal = window

qwestu = winds

selka = person

tagka = tin

tlanta = children

uruk = foreigners

vakkan = rain