Pangali: Difference between revisions

813 bytes added ,  12 September 2019
m
Tags: Mobile edit Mobile web edit
 
(34 intermediate revisions by the same user not shown)
Line 8: Line 8:
{{Infobox language
{{Infobox language
|name = Pangali
|name = Pangali
|nativename = Ńipańalusuâ,<br />Pańali
|nativename = Ńipańariâ,<br />Pańari
|pronunciation = [ŋi.ba.ŋa.lu.zʉˈʔa],<br /> [pa.ŋaˈli]
|pronunciation = [ŋi.ba.ŋa.ɹɨˈʔa],<br /> [pa.ŋaˈði]
|creator = [[User:Kóði|Sámuel Hiette Kannosíni]]<br />
|creator = [[User:Kóði|Sámuel Hiette Kannosíni]]<br />
|created = August 8th, 2019
|created = August 8th, 2019
Line 44: Line 44:
In spite of its status as a language isolate, the phonological system of Pangali isn't terribly special in comparison to its neighbors in the Philippines or Australia. A simple syllable structure, few (phonemic) vowels, a fair amount of consonants, and stable vowel quality make this language both interesting and soundly (''ha, pun!'') mundane all at the same time.
In spite of its status as a language isolate, the phonological system of Pangali isn't terribly special in comparison to its neighbors in the Philippines or Australia. A simple syllable structure, few (phonemic) vowels, a fair amount of consonants, and stable vowel quality make this language both interesting and soundly (''ha, pun!'') mundane all at the same time.
=== Phoneme Inventory ===
=== Phoneme Inventory ===
Pangali's consonant inventory weighs in at a respectable 21 phonemic consonants. A unique feature that may be noted is the presence of phonemic prenasalized stops.
Pangali's consonant inventory weighs in at a respectable 14 phonemic consonants. A unique feature that may be noted is the presence of phonemic prenasalized stops.


The vowels, on the other hand, number only three in terms of phonemic units. As with many languages with small vowel inventories, what vowels that Pangali does have are subject to significant allophonic variation.
The vowels, on the other hand, number only three in terms of phonemic units. As with many languages with small vowel inventories, what vowels that Pangali does have are subject to significant allophonic variation.
{| class="wikitable" style="margin-left:auto; margin-right: auto; border: none; text-align:center"
{| class="wikitable" style="margin-left:auto; margin-right: auto; border: none; text-align:center"
|+Pangali Consonant Inventory
|+Pangali Consonant Inventory
Line 53: Line 54:
! Labial
! Labial
! Alveolar
! Alveolar
! Palatal
! Velar
! Velar
! Glottal
! Glottal
|-
|-
! colspan=2|Nasal
! colspan=2|Nasal
| m {{angbr|m}}
|m {{angbr|m}}
| n {{angbr|n}}
|n {{angbr|n}}
| ŋ {{angbr|ń}}
|
|ŋ {{angbr|ń}}
|
|
|-
|-
! rowspan=2 | Plosive
! rowspan=2 | Plosive
! Plain
! Plain
| p {{angbr|p}}
|p {{angbr|p}}
| t {{angbr|t}}
|t {{angbr|t}}
| k {{angbr|k}}
|
| ʔ {{angbr|ˆ}}
|k {{angbr|k}}
|ʔ {{angbr|ˆ}}
|-
|-
! Prenasalized
! Prenasalized
| ᵐp {{angbr|b}}
|ᵐp {{angbr|b}}
| ⁿt {{angbr|d}}
|ⁿt {{angbr|d}}
| ᵑk {{angbr|g}}
|
|
|-
|ᵑk {{angbr|g}}
! colspan=2|Fricative
|  
|
|
|
| h {{angbr |h}}
|-
|-
! colspan=2|Approximant
! colspan=2|Approximant
|  
|  
|/ɹ/ {{angbr|r}}
|ɹ {{angbr|r}}
|/w/ {{angbr|w}}
|j {{angbr|j}}
|
|w {{angbr|w}}
|h {{angbr|h,hj}}
|-
|-


Notes:
*The alveolar plosives /t/ and /ⁿt/ are laminal.


{| class="wikitable" style="margin-left:auto; margin-right: auto; border: none; text-align:center"
{| class="wikitable" style="margin-left:auto; margin-right: auto; border: none; text-align:center"
Line 95: Line 97:
|-
|-
! Close
! Close
| /i/ {{angbr |i}}
|i {{angbr|i}}
| /u/ {{angbr |u}}
|u {{angbr|u}}
|-
|-
! Open
! Open
|/a/ {{angbr |a}}
|a {{angbr|a}}
|
|
|-
|-
Line 105: Line 107:


=== Romanization ===
=== Romanization ===
The Pangalu have no writing system and due to cultural reasons have declined offers to be taught the Romanization provided by academics. Despite this, field linguists who have begun the process of recording the language have developed a relatively intuitive Romanization system for use in records of the language.
The Pangalu currently have no official writing system but offers to be taught the romanization used in the recorded lexicon produced by academics have been met with enthusiasim.


=== Allophonic Variation ===
=== Allophonic Variation ===
==== Plosives ====
==== Plosives ====
Obstruents are the most unstable aspect of Pangali's phonological inventory, with a total of nine different allophonic realizations based on stress alone.
Comprising 12 different allophonic variations, Pangali's plosives are the most unstable aspect of the language's phonological inventory.
===== <small>Sonorization</small> =====
===== <small>Sonorization</small> =====
Standard Pangali: Plain voiceless plosives become voiced in intervocalic positions.
Standard Pangali: Plain voiceless plosives become voiced in intervocalic positions.
Line 118: Line 120:
!Example
!Example
|-
|-
!/p/ [b]
!p → b / V_V
|/ʔu'''p'''a/ → [ʔuˈ'''b'''a]
|/ʔu'''p'''a/ → [ʔuˈ'''b'''a]
|-
|-
!/t/ [d]
!t → d / V_V
|/huwu'''t'''uwi/ → [hu.wu.'''d'''uˈvi]<sup>1</sup>
|/huwu'''t'''uwi/ → [hu.wu.'''d'''uˈɣʷi]<sup>1</sup>
|-
|-
!/k/ [g]
!k → g / V_V
|/ⁿta'''k'''u/ → [ⁿtaˈ'''g'''u]
|/ⁿta'''k'''u/ → [ⁿtaˈ'''g'''u]
|-
|-
|}
|}
Notes:
*A minority of speakers only voice /p t k/ in unstressed syllables, primarily in the more southern islands.


===== <small>Dissimilation</small> =====
===== <small>Dissimilation</small> =====
[WIP]


Word initially, /p t k/ shift to [f s x] when the following syllable shares the same onset. Under the same conditions intervocalically, /p t k/ instead shift to [p͡f t͡s k͡x]:
Word initially, /p t k/ spirantize to [f s x] when the following syllable shares the same onset. Under the same conditions intervocalically, /p t k/ instead affricate to [p͡f t͡s k͡x]:
{| class="wikitable" style="float:center; border: none; text-align:center"
{| class="wikitable" style="float:center; border: none; text-align:center"
|-
|-
Line 140: Line 144:
!Example
!Example
|-
|-
!/p/ [f]
!p → f / _Vp
|/'''p'''apa/ → ['''f'''aˈba]
|/'''p'''apa/ → ['''f'''aˈba]
!/p/ [p͡f]
!p → p͡f / V_Vp
|/ᵐpi'''p'''apa/ → [ᵐpi.'''p͡f'''aˈba]
|/ᵐpi'''p'''apa/ → [ᵐpi.'''p͡f'''aˈba]
|-
|-
!/t/ [s]
!t → s / _Vt
|/'''t'''u.tiɹa/ → ['''s'''u.diˈɹa]
|/'''t'''utiɹa/ → ['''s'''u.diˈða]
!/t/ [t͡s]
!t → t͡s / V_Vt
|/ᵐpi'''t'''u.tiɹa/ → [ᵐpi.'''t͡s'''u.diˈɹa]
|/ᵐpi'''t'''utiɹa/ → [ᵐpi.'''t͡s'''u.diˈða]
|-
|-
! /k/ [x]
!k → x / _Vk
|/'''k'''uka/ → ['''x'''uˈga]
|/'''k'''uka/ → ['''x'''uˈga]
!/k/ [k͡x]
!k → k͡x / V_Vk
|/ᵐpi'''k'''uka/ → [ᵐpi.'''k͡x'''uˈga]
|/ᵐpi'''k'''uka/ → [ᵐpi.'''k͡x'''uˈga]
|-
|-
|}
|}


It is theorized that /p t k/ originally affricated in all positions but later spirantized in word initial.
Notes:
*It is theorized that /p t k/ originally affricated in all positions but later spirantized word initially.
*For the speakers from the island of ''Batawutarjikuri'' affricates continue to occur both word intially and intervocalically.


==== <small>Labio-Velar Fortition</small> ====
==== Approximants ====
/w/ fortifies to [] preceding /u/ and to [v] before /i a/ when occuring in a stressed syllable:
===== <small>Fortition</small> =====
/ɹ j w/ fortify to [ð̠ ʝ ɣʷ] when occurring in a stressed syllable:


{|class="wikitable" style="float:center; border: none; text-align:center"
{|class="wikitable" style="float:center; border: none; text-align:center"
Line 166: Line 173:
! colspan=2 | Examples
! colspan=2 | Examples
|-
|-
! rowspan=2 | /w/ → [v]  
! rowspan=2 | ɹ → ð̠
|/pa'''w'''a/ → [paˈ'''v'''a]
|/pa'''ɹ'''a/ → [paˈ'''ð̠'''a]
|-
|/ki'''ɹ'''i/ → [kiˈ'''ð̠'''i]
|-
! rowspan=2 | j → ʝ
|/iɹata'''j'''u/ → [i.ɹa.daˈ'''ʝ'''u]
|-
|-
|/ki'''w'''i/ → [kiˈ'''v'''i]
|/ni'''j'''a/ → [niˈ'''ʝ'''a]
|-
|-
! rowspan=2 | /w/ [gʷ]
! rowspan=2 | w → ɣʷ
|/iɹata'''w'''u/ → [i.ɹa.daˈ'''ɡʷ'''u]
|/i'''w'''i/ → ['''ɣʷ'''i]
|-
|-
|/ni'''w'''u/ → [niˈ''''''u]
|/ku'''w'''a/ → [kuˈ'''ɣʷ'''a]
|-
|-
|}
|}
Notes:
*The most common realization of /ɹ/ is [ð̠], a voiced alveolar non-sibilant fricative, but some speakers may realize it as the sibilant counterpart [z] or even postalveolar [ʒ].
*Speakers who realize /ɹ/ as [ʒ] typically merge [ʒ] and [ʝ] to [ʒ].
*Technically, /w/ would be better transcribed as [ɣʷ~ɣ] due to notable free variation, however [ɣʷ] is more commonly encountered and thus the more common transcription.


==== Vowels ====
==== Vowels ====


==== <small>Centralization</small> ====
===== <small>Centralization</small> =====
Vowels preceding /ʔ/ are centralized. /a/ is both centralized and raised:
Vowels preceding /ʔ/ are centralized. /a/ is both centralized and raised:


Line 188: Line 205:
!Example
!Example
|-
|-
!/a/ [ɐ]
!a → ɐ /_ʔ
|/h'''a'''ʔamu/ → [h'''ɐ'''.ʔaˈmu]<sup>2</sup>
|/h'''a'''ʔamu/ → [h'''ɐ'''.ʔaˈmu]<sup>2</sup>
|-
|-
!/u/ [ʉ]
!u → ʉ /_ʔ
|/han'''u'''ʔa/ → [ha.n'''ʉ'''ˈʔa]<sup>2</sup>
|/han'''u'''ʔa/ → [ha.n'''ʉ'''ˈʔa]<sup>2</sup>
|-
|-
!/i/ [ɨ]
!i → ɨ /_ʔ
|/p'''i'''ʔa/ → [p'''ɨ'''ˈʔa]
|/p'''i'''ʔa/ → [p'''ɨ'''ˈʔa]
|-
|-
|}
|}


==== <small>Lowering</small> ====
Notes:
*Southern speakers do not raise /a/, but rather retract it to [ɑ]
*For many speakers, [ɨ] and [ʉ] may be in free variation with [ɪ] and [ʊ], respectively.
 
===== <small>Lowering</small> =====
/i/ and /u/ are lowered to [ɛ] and [ɔ], respectively, when preceding nasals and prenasalized consonants:
/i/ and /u/ are lowered to [ɛ] and [ɔ], respectively, when preceding nasals and prenasalized consonants:


Line 207: Line 228:
!colspan=2 | Examples
!colspan=2 | Examples
|-
|-
!/i/ [ɛ]
!i → ɛ /_N
|/h'''i'''ⁿtu/ → [h'''ɛ'''ˈⁿtu] || /tat'''i'''nu/ → [sa.d'''ɛ'''ˈnu]
|/h'''i'''ⁿtu/ → [h'''ɛ'''ˈⁿtu] || /tat'''i'''nu/ → [sa.d'''ɛ'''ˈnu]
|-
|-
!/u/ [ɔ]
!u → ɔ /_N
|/ʔ'''u'''ŋa/ → [ʔ'''ɔ'''ˈŋa] || /k'''u'''maki/ → [k'''ɔ'''.maˈgi]
|/ʔ'''u'''ŋa/ → [ʔ'''ɔ'''ˈŋa] || /k'''u'''maki/ → [k'''ɔ'''.maˈgi]
|-
|-
Line 219: Line 240:


==== Syllable Structure ====
==== Syllable Structure ====
Pangali has a (C)V structure, where a syllable can only contain one vowel, which may or may not be preceded by a consonant. Word initial syllables may occur with either a null onset or with any single consonant, while word internal syllables may only occur with single consonant onsets. Codas are forbidden in any context, with all syllables being open. This is true even for geminate consonants, thus they are analyzed as single consonants as opposed to a sequence of [VC.CV].
Pangali has a (C)V structure, where a syllable can only contain one vowel, which may or may not be preceded by a consonant. Word initial syllables may occur with either a null onset or with any single consonant except for /ɹ/ and /j/.
 
Word internal syllables may only occur with single consonant onsets, with the exception of /h/, which may only appear word initially. Codas are forbidden in any context, with all syllables being open.
 
==== Epenthesis ====
==== Epenthesis ====
In instances where a word ending in a null coda (i.e. all words) is followed by a word beginning with a null onset, an epenthetic /ŋ/ is inserted to replace the null onset. This process is treated as if there is no word boundary, and thus the presence of the epenthetic nasal lowers the preceding vowel if it is high, while in other instances word boundaries would prevent this.
In instances where a word ending in a null coda (i.e. all words) is followed by a word beginning with a null onset, an epenthetic /ŋ/ is inserted to replace the null onset. This process is treated as if there is no word boundary, and thus the presence of the epenthetic nasal lowers the preceding vowel if it is high, while in other instances word boundaries would prevent this.
*/satawu/ + /ulunasa/ = [sa.daˈgʷɔ ŋu.lɔˈna.sa]
*/hatawu/ + /utunasa/ = [sa.daˈɣʷɔ ŋu..naˈða]
**/satawu/ + /ŋaᵐpinasa/ = [sa.daˈgʷu ˈŋa.ᵐpi.na.zə]
**/hatawu/ + /ŋaᵐpinaɹa/ = [sa.daˈɣʷu ŋa.ᵐpi.naˈða]


==== Loanwords ====
==== Loanwords ====
Due to the nature of this syllable structure non-conforming loanwords are often heavily altered; although it should be noted that calques are extremely more common in favor of familiarity when possible.
Due to the nature of this syllable structure non-conforming loanwords are often heavily altered; although it should be noted that calques are extremely more common in favor of familiarity when possible.
Note the insertion of /u/ to break apart clusters and avoid final consonants in the following example.
Note the simplification of clusters to the second consonant in the example below:


*Helicopter: /ˈhɛlɪˌkɑptɚ/ > '''alikuputalu'' /ʔalikuputalu/, [ˈʔa.li.gu.bu.da.lu]
*Helicopter: /ˈheliˌkɔptə(ɹ)/ →  /hiɹikuta/


==== Stress ====
==== Stress ====
Line 285: Line 309:
Pangali, in line with its use of case marking, allows its speakers to vary their choice of word order. That being said, the default and most neutral order is OSV (Object-Subject-Verb):
Pangali, in line with its use of case marking, allows its speakers to vary their choice of word order. That being said, the default and most neutral order is OSV (Object-Subject-Verb):


'''Dimumi wabuśi kasuwiśikawi'''
[ⁿtɛ.mɔˈmi|wa.ᵐpuˈɟi|ka.ɹu.wi.ɹi.ɡaˈvi]


animal-ACC father.ERG CAUS-eat
animal-ACC | father.ERG | CAUS-eat


''The father is feeding the animal''
''The father is feeding the animal''


'''Ńakukumami wabuśi iśikawi su unańalu namulinawi'''
[ŋa.k͡xu.ɡɔ.maˈmi|wa.ᵐpuˈɟi|i.ɹi.ɡaˈvi|hu|u.na.ŋaˈɡʷu|na.mu.wɛ.naˈvi]


1P.GEN-apple-ACC father.ERG CAUS-eat REL INTRANS-I fear
1P.GEN-apple-ACC | father.ERG CAUS-eat | REL | INTRANS-I | fear


''I fear that my father is eating my apple.''
''I fear that my father is eating my apple.''
375

edits