Ash: Difference between revisions
Line 448: | Line 448: | ||
|{{gloss | |{{gloss | ||
|phrase=oadas ao ammõõ | |phrase=oadas ao ammõõ bo | ||
|IPA=[ˈʔɔ̯ɑ̞ː. | |IPA=[ˈʔɔ̯ɑ̞ː.ðɐz̠‿ˈɑːʊ̯‿ʔm̩ˈmũ̯õ̞ː‿ᵐbʊ] | ||
|gloss=shine.STAT.IND-ATTR ao POSS-head | |gloss=shine.STAT.IND-ATTR ao POSS-head LOC:CRESC.STAT.IND | ||
|translation=Ao's fair hair | |translation=Ao's fair hair | ||
}} | }} |
Revision as of 14:04, 30 August 2018
Ash | |
---|---|
ahgaa | |
Pronunciation | [[Help:IPA|ˈʔɑ̞ħˌqɑ̞ː]] |
Created by | Ava Skoog |
?
| |
Introduction
Ash (ahgaa, lit. "seaspeak", pronounced [ˈʔɑ̞ħˌqɑ̞ː]) is the anglicised name of a language mostly spoken around coastal areas, notably the town of Appa (ahba). Its speakers are familiar with technological advancements such as nautical vessels and steam locomotives.
The language is mildly synthetic to polysynthetic, largely based around agglutination with fusional elements. There is a great focus on verbs, nominals being mostly uninflected, and significant pro-drop tendencies and a general focus around deixis rather than pronominal distinctions. The word order is heavily SOV.
Phonology
Phonemes
The underlying sounds of Ash are few enough that a simple listing is preferable to a traditional table:
Vocalic | /a~Ø i~j~Ø u~w~Ø/ |
---|---|
Plosive | /p~β t~ð k~ɣ/ |
Affricate | /t͡ɬ~ɬ t͡s~s/ |
Glottal | /h~ʔ/ |
Nasal | /m~˜ n~˜ ŋ~˜/ |
The reasoning for this rather unusual classification is down to phonotactic patterning: these five groups all behave somewhat differently and serve as a more useful distinction than point of articulation when describing the phonology of Ash. The pair or triplet given for each phoneme refers to an alternation between various allophonic realisations despite the relatively low number of underlying sounds, an important feature of the language that makes the variation richer on the surface. For instance, long vowels (romanised by doubling the vowel) and nasal vowels (romanised using a tilde) are not analysed as phonemic.
Romanisation
The romanisation strikes a balance between representing phonemes versus surface realisations and uses the following seventeen letters:
a | ã | e | ẽ | y | o | õ | w | b | d | g | l | s | h | m | n | ŋ |
An example of a word with its archiphonemic, phonemic and surface transcriptions as well as romanisation:
//ˈwat.ha.ku// | → /ˈwah.taˌku/ | → [ˈʔɔ̯ɑ̞ħ.t̠ɐˌɣʊ] | → oahdago "during the day" |
Syllable structure
A cluster cannot exceed two consonants and must be of one of the following configurations:
CC | Both consonants are the same |
FP | Fricative followed by plosive |
NP | Nasal followed by plosive |
PN~FN | Plosive or fricative followed by nasal |
Phonological processes
Depending on the underlying nature of a cluster, various processes may take place either on a phonemic (phoneme alternation) or on a phonetic (surface allophony) level. For example, /t/ merges with /t͡s/ on the phonemic level before /i~j/ or a plosive or an affricate, but alternates with [ð] on the phonetic level between vowels.
Alternations before glides
In addition to the aforementioned example, nasals and fricatives will geminate before consonantal glides (i.e. /j w/ when they do not surface as vocalic), assimilating to the glide and eliding it in the process.
/-j/ | /-w/ | |
---|---|---|
//N-// | [ɲ.ɲ] | [m.m] |
//h-// | [ç.ç] | [ʍ.ʍ] |
/t͡s-/ | [ɕ.ɕ] | [s̠.s̠] |
/t͡ɬ-/ |
Before a vowel corresponding to the glide, the first consonant is simply doubled and the glide unwritten (e.g. -nne- for /-N.ji-/) but before other vowels the glide remains (e.g. -nya- for /-N.ja-/).
Alternations before plosives and affricates
All plosives alternate phonemically with fricatives or affricates before another plosive or an affricate.
//-P// | |
---|---|
//p-// | /h.P/ [ħ.P] |
//k-// | |
//t-// | /t͡s.P/ [s̠.P~ɕ.P] |
Glottal allophony
The phoneme /h~ʔ/ surfaces as a fricative before vowels and glides (geminating in the latter case as outlined in the section on alternations before glides) as well as plosives but as a glottal stop before nasals and affricates.
//-P// | //-P͡F// | //-N// | |
---|---|---|---|
//h-// | [ħ.P] | [ʔ.P͡F] | /ʔ.ᴰN/ |
In addition, each plosive alternates phonemically with the glottal before a nasal, assimilating the nasal to its point of articulation.
//-N// | |
---|---|
//p-// | [ʔ.ᵇm] |
//t-// | [ʔ.ᵈn̠~ʔ.ᶡɲ] |
//k-// | [ʔ.ᶢŋ] |
Affricate alternations
Affricates are simplified to fricatives in much the same contexts as the plosives: intervocalically, before other plosives or affricates, word-finally as well as before nasals which in turn are prestopped.
Prosody and stress
Prefixes are always unstressed. Following the last stressed syllable an iambic pattern of secondary stress on every other underlyingly light syllable follows unless an underlyingly heavy syllable intervenes, resetting the pattern. In addition, unless at the end of a word, stressed syllables are forced to be heavy either by lengthening of the vowel or reduplication of the next syllable's onset consonant if they are not already underlyingly so.
Morphology
Ash does not mark words for number, person or case. With regards to syntactic patterning, only three significant word classes can be posited: verbs, nominals and adverbials. Nonetheless there is a degree of mobility between them.
Verbs
The bulk of all inflection goes on verbs, making them morphemic anchors fundamental to almost any utterance in the language. The general verb template looks as follows:
Stem | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Deixis | Trans./Poss. | Preverb | Incorp. | Class. | Root | Der. | State | Mood/Nom. | Attr. | Adv. | Enclitics |
The nominalisation slot creates a deverbal nominal and the adverbialisation slot creates an adverbial and so these two serve to change the class of the word; the possession slot is only used on deverbal nominals and not on regular verbs.
Stems
Each verb has a set of primary stems formed more or less predictably from a combination of affixes. The first stem, the stative indicative, is used as the lemma when citing words, such as oada "to shine":
Stative | Active | Translative | Causative | Passive | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Indicative | oada | oahda | oadna | oasya | oasda |
Optative | oase | oadse | oadne | oasse | oasdse |
All of these terms are to an extent ad hoc. For instance the passive stem is only used to form deverbal nominals; there is no true passive construction syntactically. Some verbs are inherently stative or active and do not have two distinct stems. The active or stative stems are the ones that are generally prone to being somewhat unpredictable, whereas the other three are formed productively.
Derived stems
Derivational suffixes can be used to extend the root and create a new set of stems, such as the frequentative -(d)s- and the potential -dãã-.
Base | Frequentative |
---|---|
baa "hand; arm" | → badsa "weave; make" |
oo "consume" | → odsa "gorge; overeat" |
doo "smoke" | → dodso "train; locomotive" |
Sometimes stems appear connected through no longer productive processes, such as ohwa "cook", related to oo "consume".
Nominals
Nominals are mostly unmarked; the main kind of affixation, while resembling case marking, results in adverbialisation, thus changing the class of the word. Nominals can however be marked for possession or be incorporated into a verb.
The possessive prefix n- can be preceded by a deictic prefix. Here are the possessed forms of mõõ "head; hair; top":
Neutral | Proximal | Distal |
---|---|---|
ammõõ | emmõõ | ommõõ |
Adverbials
Adverbials are used to denote a place, time or manner. Their formation sometimes resembles case marking or conjunctions more than traditional adverbs, but serves that role as well.
Some prominent adverbialising suffixes:
Suffix | Example | |
---|---|---|
Locative | -da | mehda "by the fire" |
Durative | -go | oadnago "in the morning" |
Benefactive | -ba | eaba "in order to see" |
Deixis
The language lacks true pronouns and due to its pro-drop tendencies commonly avoids alternatives as well. What does get commonly marked is deixis: whether something is close to or far away from the speaker or a previous referent; unspecified deixis is also possible. On nominals deixis is generally spatial while on verbs it is temporal (proximal working roughly as a present tense and distal as a non-present one); adverbial deixis can be either depending on the characteristics of the adverbial in question.
The deictic stems are as follows:
Neutral | Ø- (unmarked) |
---|---|
Proximal | e- |
Distal | o- |
Deixis occurs in the form of isolated nominals ee and oo as well as verbal and possessive prefixes e- and o-. Neutral deixis sometimes surfaces epenthetically as a- due to phonotactic constraints, but is not underlyingly explicitly marked.
Syntax
The word order is fairly strictly SOV, with adverbials generally preceding the nominals followed by the verb.
Valency
Transitivity and inversion
Transitivity is explicitly marked and through an inversion marker on the verb the roles of agent and patient can be swapped without a change in word order, the purpose of which is topicalisation, leaving the topic in the subject position.
|
|
Inversion is especially important when the subject is being omitted as person markers do not exist.
|
|
Incorporation
There is a limit on two unmarked nominal arguments of a verb. There are two ways to introduce more arguments, one of which is to incorporate the third nominal into the verb.
- ao bahbo odsãmmoyya
[ˈʔɑːʊ̯ ˈβɑ̞ħ.pʊ‿wo̞ʔˈt̠͡s̠ɑ̞̃mˌmʊʝ.ʝɐ]
ao dog DIST-TR-water-consume.CAUS.IND
Ao gave the dog water to drink
Adverbialisation
The other method is to completely remove the valency of the nominal by turning it into an adverbial, which is why this process sometimes resembles case marking.
- mehda ao bahbo odsãmmoyya
[ˈmᵇe̞ħ.t̠ɐ‿ˈʔɑːʊ̯ ˈβɑ̞ħ.pʊ‿wo̞ʔˈt̠͡s̠ɑ̞̃mˌmʊʝ.ʝɐ]
fire-LOC ao dog DIST-TR-water-consume.CAUS.IND
Ao gave the dog water to drink by the fire
Animacy
While there is no explicit marking for animacy, an underlying hierarchy ranging roughly from natural forces at the top to people and animals in the middle and inanimates at the bottom governs certain parts of the grammar. The main aspect of this hierarchy is that inanimate referents cannot act as agents which affects how transitive and inverse marking is interpreted in their presence.
Transitive | Inverse | |
---|---|---|
Animate | bahbo ahhoo "dogs eat it" | bahbo assoo "dogs are eaten" |
Inanimate | sãã ahhoo "water is drunk" | *sãã assoo (ungrammatical) |
Topicalisation
New non-verbal information is focused by fronting, i.e. introducing the word or phrase earlier in the sentence. This means that the order of subject and object might shift in order to focus on the object. When the object is inanimate inversion is not possible nor necessary, while for an animate object it is.
Normal | Fronted | |
---|---|---|
Animate | ao bahbo ohhea "Ao was looking at the dog" | bahbo ao ossea "it was the dog Ao was looking at" |
Inanimate | ao sãã ohhoo "Ao who was drinking water" | sãã ao ohhoo "it was water Ao was drinking" |
Subclauses
Relativisation is done simply by chaining phrases one after another, with no special marking. Subclauses go before main clauses, in which the deictic context is centered around the subject of the subclause.
- [oo oahdago bahbo ossoyya]1 [ee oahdago esseana]2
[ˈʔu̯oː‿ˈwɔ̯ɑ̞ħ.t̠ɐˌɣʊ ˈβɑ̞ħ.pʊ‿wʊs̠ˈs̠ʊʝ.ʝɐ‿ˈji̯eː‿ˈwɔ̯ɑ̞ħ.t̠ɐˌɣʊ‿jɪɕˈɕɛ̯ɑː.n̠ɐ]
[DIST shine.ACT.IND-DUR dog DIST-INV-consume.CAUS.IND]1 [PROX shine.ACT.IND-DUR PROX-INV-see.TRANS.IND]2
[today I saw]2 [the dog that you fed yesterday]1
Attributes
Attributive verbs are formed using the connector suffix -s and go before the noun phrase.
|
|
Locative verbs
An important part of Ash grammar is an extensive set of so called locative verbs which are used almost like a noun classification system and cover location, motion and related concepts while providing specific information about the referent at hand, such as specifying whether liquid is involved.
These are some of those verbs:
Lemma | Gloss | Semantic range |
---|---|---|
laa | LOC:STAT | General stative (indefinite or permanent) |
goo | LOC:ACT | General active (temporary or dynamic) |
sãã | LOC:LIQ | Water and other liquids |
see | LOC:AER | Air and weather |
boo | LOC:CRESC | Growth (hair, plants et c.) |
doo | LOC:PART | Particles (powder, sand, dust, smoke, spores et c.) |
mee | LOC:PYR | Fire |
Disambiguation
One function of locative verbs is to resolve potential ambiguities. When used solely for classification in its unmarked form, a locative verb is unstressed and thereby shortened, resembling a particle.
|
|
|
Further verbs can be serially connected after indicating the nature of a nominal using a locative verb.
- ao ammõõ bo oada
[ʔɑːʊ̯‿ʔm̩ˈmũ̯õ̞ː‿ᵐbo̞‿ˈwɔ̯ɑː.ðɐ]
ao POSS-head LOC:CRESC.STAT.IND shine.STAT.IND
Ao's hair is fair
Used this way they nonetheless remain verbs with the accompanying syntactic implications. Since they create subclauses, a nominal specified for category with a locative verb cannot be used in object position and so will always precede any agent.
- emmõõ bo ao ehbadsa
[ʔɪmˈmũ̯õ̞ː‿ᵐbo̞‿ˈʔɑːʊ̯‿je̞ħˈpɑʔ.t̠͡s̠ɐ]
PROX-POSS-head LOC:CRESC.STAT.IND ao PROX-TR-hand.FREQ.ACT.IND
Ao is braiding my hair
Since the locative verbs are only necessary when introducing new information, this ties neatly into the established system of topicalisation by fronting and so the net effect is that this limitation does not make much of a difference to normal syntax. Nominals can then be unambiguously reüsed without the classifying verb as the information is thenceforth known from the previously established context. Note that if the classified nominal had been animate in the above example (mõõ is not) inversion would have been necessary in order to specify it as the patient rather than the agent as usual.
Specification
To denote motion, an andative ("going") or venitive ("coming") prefix is placed into the verbal classifier slot.
|
|
The preverb slot can be used to specify manner, location or direction.
|
|
Use with adverbials
Adverbial location is generic and locative verbs can be used to specify the meaning.
|
|