Nankôre: Difference between revisions

From Linguifex
Jump to navigation Jump to search
(Filled in the missing Gloss entries and a lot of rework on "yôripoh" and "yôra' ")
 
(487 intermediate revisions by 2 users not shown)
Line 4: Line 4:
|name          = Nankôre
|name          = Nankôre
|nativename    = Nankôre
|nativename    = Nankôre
|pronunciation = /nan.'ko:re/
|pronunciation = nan.'ko:re
|region        = [[wikipedia:Pacific Northwest|Pacific Northwest]]
|setting      = [[wikipedia:Pacific Northwest|Pacific Northwest]]
|states        = Nanhoshka Kôya  
|states        = Nanhoska Kôya /nan'hoʃka 'ko:ja/
|speakers      = 3,232,430
|ethnicity    = Nanhoska people
|speakers      = 2,324
|date          = 2014
|date          = 2014
|familycolor  = panlaffic
|familycolor  = panlaffic
|fam1          = Nahenic
|fam1          = Nahenic
|fam2          = Nankoric
|fam2         = Shakhtabaric
|fam3         = Nankoric
|nation        = Koya Island
|nation        = Koya Island
|iso1          = nk
|script1      = Latn
|iso2          = nnk
|creator      = User:Anyar
|script        = [[wikipedia:Latin alphabet|Latin alphabet]] (Nankore)
}}
}}
<!--  
<!--  
Line 39: Line 40:




==Background==
==Introduction==


Nankôre, from the words ''nan'' ("man, human") and ''kôre'' ("speech"), is spoken by the Nanhoshka people of Kôya Island.  There are two main dialects, the Konishmak, located in the Northeastern Coastal Mountains, and the Sapshira, encompassing the western and southern parts of the country.  The dialects differ chiefly in pronunciation and differences in certain lexical items, but are otherwise mutually intelligible.
Nankôre, from the words ''nan'' ("man, human") and ''kôre'' ("speech"), is spoken by the Nanhoshka people of Kôya Island.  There are two main dialects, the Konishmak, located in the Northeastern Coastal Mountains, and the Sapshira, encompassing the western and southern parts of the country.  The dialects differ chiefly in pronunciation and differences in certain lexical items, but are otherwise mutually intelligible.


For a little over a century the origins of the language and its people remain clouded in mystery.  Physically, the Nanhoshka people look distinct from the Northwest Pacific Native Americans just 862 kilometers from the eastern shore, but their physical features bear a strikingly close resemblance to the [[Minhast]] people of the Republic of Minhay, who live much further away on the other side of the Pacific Ocean close to Japan and Ainushir.  Because of these physical resemblances, both anthropologists and linguists started exploring a possible relationship between the two peoples.  Recent genetic research uncovered that both the Nanhoshka and Minhast belong to the haplogroup C-M212, indicating a common ancestry.  Linguists had a more difficult task in establishing a relationship.  Finally Benson et. al. compiled an extensive Swadesh list and internally consistent sound correspondences between the two languages.  Most importantly, a set of affixes that were quite conservative were revealed, notably the Causative affix, and a set of fossilized affixes which are prefixed to body parts to derive verbs.  In addition, a third language, [[Nahónda]], also previously classified as a language isolate, was discovered to contain common vocabulary and affixes which it shared with both Nankôre and Minhast.  Therefore, both Nankôre and Minhast, along with Nahónda, are now recognized as belonging to the same language family, which has been named the Nahenic language family.
For a little over a century the origins of the language and its people remain clouded in mystery.  Physically, the Nanhoshka people look distinct from the Northwest Pacific Native Americans just 862 kilometers from the eastern shore, but their physical features bear a strikingly close resemblance to the [[Minhast]] people of the Minhastim Karak, who live much further away on the other side of the Pacific Ocean close to Japan and Moshir Ainu.  Because of these physical resemblances, both anthropologists and linguists started exploring a possible relationship between the two peoples.  Recent genetic research uncovered that both the Nanhoshka and Minhast belong to the haplogroup C-M212, indicating a common ancestry.  Linguists had a more difficult task in establishing a relationship.  Finally Benson et. al. compiled an extensive Swadesh list and internally consistent sound correspondences between the two languages.  Most importantly, a set of affixes that were quite conservative were revealed, notably the Causative affix, and a set of fossilized affixes which are prefixed to body parts to derive verbs.  In addition, a third language, [[Nahónda]], also previously classified as a language isolate, was discovered to contain common vocabulary and affixes which it shared with both Nankôre and Minhast.  Therefore, both Nankôre and Minhast, along with Nahónda, are now recognized as belonging to the same language family, which has been named the Nahenic language family.
 
==Typology and Grammatical Overview==
Like Minhast and Nahónda, Nankôre is an SOV language. However, unlike its relatives, Nankôre is not a polysynthetic language but is relatively sparse in terms of affixes for determining syntactic relations. Nouns do not receive case or number marking, nor are they marked for gender. Word order within the verb complex is rather strict, but core, patient, and obliques may occur in various orders. The verb complex consists minimally of a main verb, which possesses suffixes for marking evidentiality, and the auxiliary, which contain a rich set of affixes to mark the core arguments for various syntactic features: number, tense, voice, agency, etc.
 
Nankôre displays a mixed morphosyntactic alignment.  Particularly noticeable is the Hierarchical alignment in its transitive verbs, which  employ [https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Direct–inverse_alignment Direct-Inverse marking] to indicate core argument roles.  The nominal hierarchy is  complex; at least ten noun classes based on animacy can be identified, and within those classes sub-hierarchies are observed based on semantic features, although discourse processes (e.g. the introduction of new arguments that are roughly equal in animacy with previously established arguments, or a previous argument that serves as the topic over extended discourse) may affect how direct-inverse marking is expressed.  Thus hierarchical relations are primarily dependent on the semantics of the core argument and the verb class, although other factors are at play; native speakers intuit the hierarchy based on a complex interaction between the both the noun and verb classes, discourse patterns, and context.
 
The language also displays an Active-Stative alignment of the [https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Active–stative_alignment Split-S subtype] in its intransitive verbs. As in its relative Nahónda, Nankôre  intransitive verbs are split into two categories, one that requires an Agent as the sole core argument, and another that takes only Patients.  Two different auxiliary paradigms are used for intransitive verb roots, the  [[Nankôre#Intransitive-Active_Paradigm|''itá'']] paradigm for agentive verbs, and the  [[Nankôre#Intransitive-Stative_Paradigm|''iná'']] paradigm for stative verbs.


==Phonology==
==Phonology==
Line 49: Line 57:
<!--Explain your conlang's alphabet. Use the International Phonetic Alphabet to describe the sounds of your language. If you are unsure on how to use IPA then visit: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/International_Phonetic_Alphabet and read up. -->
<!--Explain your conlang's alphabet. Use the International Phonetic Alphabet to describe the sounds of your language. If you are unsure on how to use IPA then visit: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/International_Phonetic_Alphabet and read up. -->


The Nankôre orthography is based on the Allen-Mills system, derived from the Latin alphabet.  However, a second system, the American Media system, has started displacing the Allen-Mills system.  Although based on the Allen-Mills system, the American Media system uses the grapheme <y>, and the digraphs <ch> and <sh>, to make transcription of the language more accessible to American readers.
The Nankôre orthography is based on the Allen-Mills system, derived from the Latin alphabet.  However, a second system, the American Media system, has started displacing the Allen-Mills system.  Although based on the Allen-Mills system, the American Media system uses the the digraphs <ch> and <sh> to make transcription of the language more accessible to American readers.


{| border="1" cellpadding="1" cellspacing="1" class="bluetable lightbluebg" style="text-align:center;"
{| border="1" cellpadding="1" cellspacing="1" class="bluetable lightbluebg" style="text-align:center;"
Line 60: Line 68:
| ʔ
| ʔ
|-
|-
| a,ā, â, á
| a,ā, â
| a,ā,â, á
| a,ā,â
| a, a:
| a, a:
|-
|-
Line 88: Line 96:
| s, s̺, ʃ
| s, s̺, ʃ
|-
|-
| š
| ś, š<ref>Found mostly in older linguistic texts before 1965.  The character has largely fell into disuse afterwards.</ref>
| sh
| sh
| ʃ
| ʃ
Line 94: Line 102:
| h
| h
| h
| h
| h
| ɦ
|-
|-
| k
| k
Line 126: Line 134:




Note that the grapeme &lt;s&gt; in the Allen-Mills system is pronounced /ʃ/ or /s̺/ when followed by [i] (e.g. ''sinkokah'' /ʃin'kokah/ "orca"), or in coda position (''Nanhoska'' /nan'hoʃka/ "the True People"). A few decades after the Allen-Mills transcription system was developed, a sound shift in the northwestern Hoyampe dialect, whereby /s/ shifted to the voiceless apico-alveolar fricative  /s̺/ when followed by /i/ or in coda position.  A similar sound shift occurred at around the same time in the northeastern Konishmak dialect (possibly the result of the Hoyampe sound shift), but it was the voiceless palato-alveolar fricative /ʃ/ that appeared.  It was this sound shift that spread out through most of the country, although there are some remote regions of the country that have not been affected by either sound shift.  As a result in foreign transcriptions &lt;sh&gt; is often used in words where the /s/ &gt;&gt;  /ʃ/ has already occurred, e.g. ''akôsh'' vs. the original ''akôs'' transcription
Note that the grapeme &lt;s&gt; in the Allen-Mills system is pronounced /ʃ/ or /s̺/ when followed by [i] (e.g. ''sinkokah'' /ʃin'kokah/ "orca"), or in coda position (''Nanhoska'' /nan'hoʃka/ "the True People"). A few decades after the Allen-Mills transcription system was developed, a sound shift appeared in the northwestern Hoyampe dialect, whereby /s/ shifted to the voiceless apico-alveolar fricative  /s̺/ when followed by /i/ or in coda position.  A similar sound shift occurred at around the same time in the northeastern Konishmak dialect (possibly the result of the Hoyampe sound shift), but it was the voiceless palato-alveolar fricative /ʃ/ that appeared.  It was this sound shift that spread out through most of the country, although there are some remote regions of the country that have not been affected by either sound shift.  As a result in foreign transcriptions &lt;sh&gt; is often used in words where the /s/ &gt;&gt;  /ʃ/ has already occurred, e.g. ''akôsh'' vs. the original ''akôs'' transcription.  The grapheme &lt;ô&gt; is used to indicate vowel length for /o:/, and &lt;ā&gt; for /ɑ:/.


The grapheme &lt;ô&gt; is used to indicate vowel length for /o/, and &lt;ā&gt; for /ɑ/.
Although the American Media orthography system is the most widely used system, the Allen-Mills has long been used in academic publications, and will be used throughout the rest of this article, unless otherwise noted.
 
Because the American Media orthography system is the most widely used system, it will be used throughout the rest of this article, unless otherwise noted.


===Consonants===
===Consonants===
Nankôre has a small inventory of consonants, only fourteen in number.  Noteworthy is the paucity of voiced consonants which contributes to the limited number of consonants in the Nankôre language.  The following table contains the entire consonantal inventory:
Nankôre has a small inventory of consonants, only thirteen in number.  Noteworthy is the paucity of voiced consonants which contributes to the limited number of consonants in the Nankôre language.  The following table contains the entire consonantal inventory:


{| border="1" cellpadding="1" cellspacing="1" class="bluetable lightbluebg" style="width: 660px; text-align:center;"
{| border="1" cellpadding="1" cellspacing="1" class="bluetable lightbluebg" style="width: 660px; text-align:center;"
Line 172: Line 178:
|
|
|  
|  
| ͡tʃ
| t͡ʃ
|  
|  
|   
|   
Line 185: Line 191:
|  
|  
|   
|   
| h
| ɦ
|-
|-
! style="" |Approximant
! style="" |Approximant
Line 200: Line 206:
|  
|  
|  
|  
| r
| ɾ
|  
|  
|
|
Line 213: Line 219:
===Prosody===
===Prosody===
====Stress====
====Stress====
====Intonation====
===Phonotactics===
===Phonotactics===
<!-- Explain the consonant clusters and vowel clusters that are permissible for use in the language. For example, "st" is an allowed consonant cluster in English while onset "ng" isn't. -->
<!-- Explain the consonant clusters and vowel clusters that are permissible for use in the language. For example, "st" is an allowed consonant cluster in English while onset "ng" isn't. -->
Line 226: Line 230:


===Nouns===
===Nouns===
====Number ====
Nouns do not inflect for number, which is usually determined by context, but a numeral joined by the Connective ''si='' to its head noun can be used to indicate plurality, eg:
{{Gloss
|phrase = ôhi sikurasno.
| IPA = /'o:ɦi ʃi ku'raʃno/
| morphemes = ôhi si=kurasno
| gloss = two CONN girl
| translation = two girls
}}
If the noun is a core argument, then the  [[Nankôre#The_Auxiliary|auxiliary ]]  obligatorily marks for number.  Number is indicated on the auxiliary by the affix ''-n-''.  For plural Agents, it is infixed, and for plural Patients, it is suffixed.  Additionally, if the auxiliary is marked with the Inverse prefix, the prefix redundantly encodes number by vowel length, eg ''ta-'' for the singular, and ''tā='' for the plural. Some examples are given below: 


==== The Nominal Hierarchy ====
Example of a HA singular Agent acting on a LA singular Patient:  
Nankôre nouns, at first glance, appears very simple. They are not inflected for case, gender, nor do adpositions indicate their directional or positional relationships.  Number is distinguished only in pronouns. Nevertheless, the nominal system has a complex hierarchy of animacy, which although unmarked, is a semantic feature of the noun and pronoun. This hierarchy of animacy is used to determine the syntactic roles of the core arguments of a clause, namely that of Agent and Patient.  Arguments that are higher in the Animacy Hierarchy are by default considered Agents, whereas lower animacy arguments are considered Patients.  If the syntactic roles of two core arguments are reversed, namely a lower animacy argument acts as an Agent on a higher animacy Patient argument, a special construction known as the Inverse Voice, involving the prefix ''tā(h)-'', is added to both the first verb in the verb phrase and the auxiliary verb ''itá'''. Thus, the Inverse Voice is doubly-marked.


Where a given NP falls within the animacy hierarchy must be memorized in order to use the verbal inverse prefix ''tā(h)-'' correctly (see section on [[Nankôre#Verbs|Verbs]] for additional details regarding inverse marking).
{{Gloss
|phrase = kurasno rompóy rayrokor nitá'.
| IPA = /ku'raʃno rom'poɪ 'raɪrokor nɪ'taʔ/
| morphemes = kurasno rompóy Ø-rayro=kor Ø-ni-Ø-itá-ʔ
| gloss = girl.HA dog.LA DIR=play=EVID.HEAR DIR-PST-HS-COP-LS
| translation = I heard that the girl played with the dog recently.
}}


The Nankôre nominal hierarchy has been traditionally classified along two axes, the first marking the noun class, and the second marking levels of hierarchy within a given class.  Animacy along classes decrease as one traverses from Supernatural/Phenomonological onwards towards the Ephemeral.  Animacy decreases as one goes down the Level axis.  The hierarchy is arranged according to the following table:
Example of a HA plural Agent acting on a LA singular Patient:  


{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
{{Gloss
|+ '''Table of Nominal Animacy hierarchy'''
|phrase = kurasno rompóy rayrokor nintá'.
|-
| IPA = /ku'raʃno rom'poɪ 'raɪrokor nɪn'taʔ/
!
| morphemes = kurasno rompóy Ø-rayro-kor Ø-ni-n-itá-'
! colspan="10" |Noun Class
| gloss = girl.HA dog.LA play-EVID.HEAR DIR-PST-HP-COP-LS
|-
| translation = I heard that the girls played with the dog recently.
!  rowspan="2" | Level
}}
!  rowspan="2" | Supernatural &<br/>Phenomonological
 
Example of a LA singular Agent acting on a HA plural Patient:
 
{{Gloss
|phrase = rompóy kurasno tarayrokor tānintá'.
| IPA = /romp'oɪ ku'raʃno ta'raɪrokor 'ta:nɪntáʔ/
| morphemes = rompóy kurasno ta=rayro=kor tā-ni-n-itá-ʔ
| gloss = dog.LA girl.HA INV=play-HEAR INV.LS-PST-HP-COP-LS
| translation = I heard that the dog played with the girls recently.
}}
 
Example of a LA plural Agent acting on a HA plural Patient:
 
{{Gloss
|phrase = rompóy kurasno tarayrokor tānintan.
| IPA = /romp'oɪ ku'raʃno ta'raɪrokor 'ta:nɪntan/
| morphemes = rompóy kurasno ta=rayro=kor tā-ni-n-itá-n
| gloss = dog.LA girl.HA INV=play=EVID.HEAR INV.PL-PST-HP-COP-LP
| translation = I heard that the dogs played with the girls recently.
}}
 
 
==== The Nominal Hierarchy ====
Nankôre nouns, at first glance, appears very simple. They are not inflected for case, number, or gender, nor do adpositions indicate their directional or positional relationships.  Number is distinguished only in pronouns. Nevertheless, the nominal system has a complex hierarchy of animacy, which although unmarked, is a semantic feature of the noun and pronoun. This hierarchy of animacy is used to determine the syntactic roles of the core arguments of a clause, namely that of Agent and Patient.  Arguments that are higher in the Animacy Hierarchy are by default considered Agents, whereas lower animacy arguments are considered Patients.  If the syntactic roles of two core arguments are reversed, namely a lower animacy argument acts as an Agent on a higher animacy Patient argument, a special construction known as the Inverse Voice, involving the prefix ''tā-/ta-'', is added to both the first verb in the verb phrase and the auxiliary verb ''itá'''.  Thus, the Inverse Voice is doubly-marked.
 
Where a given NP falls within the animacy hierarchy must be memorized in order to use the verbal inverse prefix ''tā-/ta-'' correctly (see section on [[Nankôre#Verbs|Verbs]] for additional details regarding inverse marking).
 
The Nankôre nominal hierarchy has been traditionally classified along two axes, the first marking the noun class, and the second marking levels of hierarchy within a given class.  Animacy along classes decrease as one traverses from Supernatural/Phenomonological onwards towards the Ephemeral.  Animacy decreases as one goes down the Level axis.  The following table illustrates the animacy relations between the noun hierarchies, although in reality, '''animacy is more complex than the table would suggest''', as discourse and other contextual elements may influence a native speaker's expression of the hierarchy in a given utterance.
 
 
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
|+ '''Table of Nominal Animacy hierarchy'''
|-
!
! colspan="11" |Noun Class
|-
!  rowspan="2" | Level
!  rowspan="2" | Supernatural &<br/>Phenomonological
!  rowspan="2" | Human
!  rowspan="2" | Human
!  colspan="2"  | Animals
!  colspan="2"  | Animals
!  rowspan="2" | Weather
!  rowspan="2" | Lower Animals
!  rowspan="2" |Lower Animals
!  rowspan="2" | Riverine
!  rowspan="2" |Weather
!  rowspan="2"| Plants
!  rowspan="2"| Plants
! rowspan="2"|Geophysical
! rowspan="2"|Geophysical  
! rowspan="2"|Material
! rowspan="2"|Material
!  rowspan="2" |Ephemeral
!  rowspan="2" |Ephemeral
Line 258: Line 317:
| Adults >> Children
| Adults >> Children
| colspan="2" style="text-align:center"| predator >> prey
| colspan="2" style="text-align:center"| predator >> prey
|  Clouds
|  Reptiles (excluding snakes)
|  Reptiles (excluding snakes)
|  Rushing rivers
|  Clouds
|  Forests
|  Forests
Rushing rivers
Mountains
Water
Liquid, usually in a container
|  Magic
|  Magic
|-  
|-  
Line 269: Line 329:
| Older >> Younger
| Older >> Younger
| colspan="2" style="text-align:center"| wild >> domesticated
| colspan="2" style="text-align:center"| wild >> domesticated
|  Slow-moving rivers
|  Rain
|  Rain
|  Snakes
|  Snakes
|  Large Trees
|  Large Trees
Slow-moving rivers
Glaciers
|  Food
|  Food
|  Actions
|  Actions
Line 280: Line 341:
| Male >> Female
| Male >> Female
| colspan="2" style="text-align:center"| Pet (usually carnivorous) >> Non-Pet (usually raised for food)
| colspan="2" style="text-align:center"| Pet (usually carnivorous) >> Non-Pet (usually raised for food)
| Amphibians
| Streams <br/> Brooks <br/> Springs
| Snow
| Snow
| Amphibians
| Small Trees
| Small Trees
| Streams, brooks, springs
| Cliffs<br/> Jagged outcroppings <br/> Tumuli <br/> Boulders
| Fabrics, fibers, skins, clothing
| Fabrics<br/> Fibers <br/> Skins <br/> Clothing
| Emotions
| Emotions
|-  
|-  
! 4
! 4
| Destructive Forces of Nature
| Destructive Forces of Nature
| colspan="3" style="text-align:center"| Large >> Small
| colspan="3" style="text-align:center"| Large >> Small
| Fish
| rowspan="5" style="text-align:center"|Lakes
| Wind
| Wind
| Fish
| Fruit/Nut Trees
| Fruit/Nut Trees
| Lakes
| rowspan="5" style="text-align:center"| Flat land <br/> Gently rolling landscape <br/> Plains
| Wood  
| Wood  
| Thoughts
| Thoughts
Line 302: Line 365:
| Breeze
| Breeze
| Crustaceans
| Crustaceans
| Bushes/Shrubs
| Bushes<br/>Shrubs
| Glaciers
| Buildings
| Buildings
| Disease
| Disease
Line 310: Line 372:
| Ocean
| Ocean
| colspan="3" style="text-align:center"| Quick >> Slow
| colspan="3" style="text-align:center"| Quick >> Slow
| Water
| Insects
| Insects
| Moving Water
| Prairie grass
| Prairie grass
| Mountains
| Metals  
| Metals  
| States
| States
Line 321: Line 382:
| Intelligent >> Unintelligent
| Intelligent >> Unintelligent
| colspan="2" style="text-align:center"| Carnivore >> Herbivore >> Carrion Eater
| colspan="2" style="text-align:center"| Carnivore >> Herbivore >> Carrion Eater
| Worms
| Air
| Air
| Worms
| Leafy plants
| Leafy plants
| Cliffs, jagged outcroppings, tumuli, boulders
| Jewels and gemstones
| Jewels and gemstones
| Abstractions
| Abstractions
Line 333: Line 393:
| style="text-align:center"|Stud >> Gelded
| style="text-align:center"|Stud >> Gelded
| style="text-align:center"|Flight >> Flightless
| style="text-align:center"|Flight >> Flightless
| Mollusks
| Muggy weather
| Muggy weather
| Mollusks
| Mushrooms <br/>Fungi<br/>Mosses<br/>Gelatinous sea invertebrates
| Mushrooms/Fungi/Mosses
| Flat land or gently rolling landscape
| Stones, rocks, pebbles
| Stones, rocks, pebbles
| Death
| Death
Line 342: Line 401:




If two nouns with equal animacy but different number appear as core arguments, the plural noun is assigned a higher animacy status than the singular noun.  However, if animacy and number are equal, the most recently introduced noun, providing it was introduced as a core argument, is assigned the higher animacy status.
If two nouns with equal animacy but different number (obtained either through pronominal referents or context) appear as core arguments, the plural noun is assigned a higher animacy status than the singular noun.  However, if animacy and number are equal, the most recently introduced noun, providing it was introduced as a core argument, is assigned the higher animacy status.


In situations were two nouns have equal animacy ''and'' equal number, the rules for assigning the position each core argument in the hierarchy is as follows:
In situations were two nouns have equal animacy ''and'' equal number, the rules for assigning the position each core argument in the hierarchy is as follows:


# If one of the nouns is just newly introduced info, it is considered less animate because the older noun has been presumably been the center of discourse and at least during part of the previous discourse it was agent. SOV order is  usually used, particularly if pragmatics and other contextual cues cannot disambiguate the syntactic roles
# If one of the nouns is newly introduced information, it is considered less animate because the older noun has been presumably been the center of discourse and at least during part of the previous discourse it was agent. SOV order is  usually used, particularly if pragmatics and other contextual cues cannot disambiguate the syntactic roles
# HOWEVER, if the new noun is topicalized with the clitic ="=hat", it is considered the agent.  
# HOWEVER, if the new noun is topicalized with the clitic ''=si'', it is considered the agent.  
# Another pattern, where a new noun is introduced by an intransitive been as a Patient argument, it is regarded as the less animate argument once the older noun is reintroduced back into the discourse in a transitive construction.   
# Another pattern, where a new noun is introduced as a Patient argument, it is regarded as the less animate argument once the older noun is reintroduced back into the discourse in a transitive construction.   
# If the animacy level of each argument cannot be resolved, then the unmarked SOV word order is used when context can't disambiguate syntactic roles.
# If the animacy level of each argument cannot be resolved, then the unmarked SOV word order is used when context can't disambiguate syntactic roles.


==== The Noun Template ====
==== Derivational ====


root - derivation - adjective - demonstrative/decessives
* -no: Diminutive suffix, e.g. ''kuras'' (woman), ''kurasno'' (girl)
* -CV(C) reduplication: Augmentive, e.g. ''kurasras'' (big woman); analogous to Minhast augmentation
* -rV-CV(C) infixation + reduplication: Augmentive-Deprecative: ''nahón'' -> ''nannarohono'', from ''na~na~ro-hón-ho'' (big/fat ugly man); similar to Minhast ''-ra-'' infixation and reduplication of first syllable, e.g. Minhast ''kas~ka~ra-slub'' "large dog, hunting hound" < ''kaslub'' "dog"


===== Derivational Affixes=====
==== Adjectival Suffixes ====
Nankôre lacks a separate word class for adjectives.  They may be expressed as attributive verbs, e.g. ''Aryak neshkak itá'', "Aryak is smart", ''Kanko shoykar sip'itá''', "Kanko (an indigenous vegetable) makes good" (for your health), or a suffix may be attached to the noun it modifies.  These suffixes are called adjectival suffixes.  Some of these suffixes are etymologically derived from an attributive verb, e.g. ''karyak itá'' > ''-kerek'' >, as in ''Kurasnokerek '' "the girl is red" (i.e. the girl is blushing).  Others appear to be derived from separate roots, c.f. "''Aryak-arku''", ("clever Aryak") vs ''"Aryak neshkak itá"'' ("Aryak is clever").  Theoretically an unlimited number of suffixes may be appended to the noun, but in practice the number of adjectival suffixes seldom exceeds three.  The  suffixes may appear in any order, so long as the meaning is comprehensible.  Scope ordering determines the position of the suffixes relative to each other, with those of wider scope appearing towards the end, as in the following example:


-no: Diminutive suffix, e.g. ''kuras'' (woman), ''kurasno'' (girl)
{{Gloss
|phrase = Kenenkorpitasnune iná'.
| IPA = /kɛnɛnkorpɪtaʃ'nunɛ ɪ'naʔ/
| morphemes = kenenkor-pita-snu-ne iná
| gloss = bed-plush-red-soft-very COP
| translation = It is a very soft, red and plush bed.
}}
 
While the number of adjectival suffixes are considerable, they remain for the most part a closed class. Moreover, with a few exceptions, each adjectival suffix may be expressed by a corresponding stative verb.
 
{{Gloss
|phrase = Hosiari sikenenkorpitasnune iná torocak ta'itāhi.
| IPA = /ho'ʃari sɪkɛnɛnkorpɪtaʃ'nunɛ ɪ'naʔ torot͡ʃak taʔɪt'a:hi/
| morphemes = hosiari si-kenenkor-pita-snu-ne iná ta-horoci=ak ta-itá'-hi
| gloss = PROX.DEM CONN-bed-plush-red-soft-very COP INV-sleep=1S+3S INV-AUX-CAUS
| translation = This very soft, red and plush bed makes me sleep (well).
}}


===== Adjectival Suffixes =====
Nankôre lacks a separate word class for adjectives.  They may be expressed as attributive verbs, e.g. ''Aryak neshkak itá'', "Aryak is smart", ''Kanko shoykar itá'', "Kanko (an indigenous vegetable) is good" (for your health), or a suffix may be attached to the noun it modifies.  These suffixes are called adjectival suffixes.  Some of these suffixes are etymologically derived from an attributive verb, e.g. ''-kerek'' > ''karyak itá'' "to be red".  Others appear to be derived from separate roots, c.f. "''Aryak-arku''", ("clever Aryak") vs ''"Aryak neshkak itá"'' ("Aryak is clever").  Theoretically an unlimited number of suffixes may be appended to the noun, but in practice the number of adjectival suffixes seldom exceeds three.  The  suffixes may appear in any order, so long as the meaning is comprehensible.  Nevertheless it appears that scope determines the ordering of the suffixes: suffixes with wider scope tend to appear towards the end e.g. "''Kenenkor-pita-shune-une itá''" (bed plush-red-soft-very 3S.COP ) "It is a very soft, red and plush bed."


There are seven observed categories of adjectival affixes:
There are seven observed categories of adjectival suffixes:


# Quality
# Quality
Line 372: Line 448:
# Other Attributes
# Other Attributes


===== Demonstrative Clitics =====
===Demonstratives and Deictics===
 
===== Spatial =====
{|  class="bluetable lightbluebg"
{|  class="bluetable lightbluebg"
|-
|-
! Type !! Spatial !! English Translation !! Example
! Type !! Independent !! Clitic !! Translation !! Example || Nahenic Cognates
|-
|-
| '''Proximal '''|| =skoro || this one near me; now || nan=iskoro "this man here"; oriyat=hepi "today"
! '''Proximal '''
| hosiairi <br/> /ho'ʃjaɪɾi/
| =skoro
|  
* this one near me
* now
|  
*  ''hosiari sinán <br/>nan=iskoro'' "this man here"  
* ''oriyat=hepi'' "today"
|
* Minhast Gull Speaker dialect: <br/>''-nussar-''
* Nahónda: ''nótsalo''
|-
|-
| '''Medio-Proximal''' || =hori, =ori || that one by you; just awhile ago|| nan=hori "that man next to you";
! '''Medio-Proximal'''
| yaiyairi
| =hori <br/> =ori  
|  
* that one by you
* just awhile ago
|  
* ''yaiyairi sinán<br/> nan=hori'' "that man next to you"  
|
* Minhast Gull Speaker dialect: <br/>''-eyyar-, -yyar-''
* Nahónda: ''iyáyalo''
|-
|-
| '''Distal''' || =nko, =kekori || yonder, that one near him/her/them; yesterday, a few days ago|| nan=inko "yonder man""
! '''Distal'''
| paipairi
| =nko <br/> =kekori  
|  
* yonder, that one near him/her/them  
* yesterday, a few days ago
|  
*''paipairi sinán'' <br/>''nan=inko'' "yonder man"
|
* Minhast Gull Speaker dialect: <br/>''-ppeyyar-''
* Nahónda: ''pʼáyalo
|-
|-
| '''Invisible''' || =nanak || that one beyond the horizon, or occluded by a distant object|| nan=nanak "that man (e.g. on the other side of the mountain)"
! '''Invisible'''
|}
| rosiairi <br/> /ɾoʃ'jaɪɾi/
|  
=nanak  
|
* that one beyond the horizon, <br/>or occluded by a distant object
|  
* ''rosiairi sinán <br/> nan=nanak'' "that man <br/>(e.g. on the other side <br/> of the mountain)"
|
* Minhast Gull Speaker dialect: <br/>''-ruššar-''
|}


<!--
<!--
Line 393: Line 511:
| Proximal || =shkoro || =hepi || this one near me; now || nan=ishkoro "this man here"; oriyat=hepi "today"
| Proximal || =shkoro || =hepi || this one near me; now || nan=ishkoro "this man here"; oriyat=hepi "today"
|-
|-
| Medio-Proximal || =hori || =enmosh || that one by you; just awhile ago|| nan=hori "that man next to you";
| Medio-Proximal || =hori || =enmosh || that one by you; just awhile ago|| nan=hori "that man next to you"  
|-
|-
| Distal || =nko || =kekori || yonder, that one near him/her/them; yesterday, a few days ago|| nan=inko "yonder man"; oriyat=kekori "yesterday", ono=kekori "a few days ago"
| Distal || =nko || =kekori || yonder, that one near him/her/them; yesterday, a few days ago|| nan=inko "yonder man"; oriyat=kekori "yesterday", ono=kekori "a few days ago"
|-
|-
| Invisible || =nanak || =sinkar || a long time ago|| nan=nanak "that man (e.g. on the other side of the mountain)"; oriyat=sinkar "a long time ago"
| Invisible || =nanak || =sinkar || a long time ago|| nan=nanak "that man <br/>(e.g. on the other side of the mountain)"; oriyat=sinkar "a long time ago"
|}
|}
-->
-->


====Number ====
===== Temporal =====
Number is usually determined by context, but plurality can be indicated by either joining a cardinal number, a quantifier, or the plural marker ''no'' before the nounThe noun is always linked to these modifiers by the ''si-'' connective, as in ''no si-kurashno'' "the girls".
The Temporal Deitic particles usually occur at the head of a sentence, e.g. ''Antak Anut Amerika san-kamuk hitá' '', "A year ago, Anut flew to America".  In multiclausal sentences, the clause in which they appear have scope over the all other clauses following it. At least one, ''yorai'', cannot appear after the initial clauseSome particles also have a clitic form which can attach to the main verb, as in ''Anut Amerika san-kamuk-entak hitá'''.
 
<!--
===Quantifiers===
, and in the process replace the auxiliary's tense affixes of the Direct Voice, as in ''Anut America ta=san-kamuk=inkát itá' '' "Several years ago Anut flew to America" (note that the Temporal Deictic clitic ''=inkát'' has replaced the past tense affix ''tā-'' in ''tā'itá' ''). The absence of these particles contrasts with the Direct Voice Distal Past form ''suphitá' '', in ''Anut America san-kamuk suphitá' '', "Anut went to America a while back". Also note that the other verb voices do not lose their tense markers, as in ''Otok erashi ta-karok=inkat ta'itá''' (boy crab INV-eat=several.years.ago INV.PST-COP) "The crab ate the boy."
 
-->
 
 
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
! Quantifier
! Substantive
! Attributive
|-
|-
! All
!type
| kamun
! Independent
| kamun si=
! Clitic
! Meaning
! Example
! Nahenic Cognates
|-
|-
! Most
! rowspan="7"| Past
| toman
| yoráy || =yoráy || Once upon a time; back then || Example ||
| toman si=
|-
|-
! Some
| inkát ||=(i)nkát || Several years ago  || Example ||
| cene    <!-- cf Minhast 'ikyem' -->
| cisi=
|-
|-
! Many
| šeyhori || =šeyhor || A few years ago || Example ||
| etco
|-
| etcisi=
| antak || =(e)ntak ||A year ago ||  Example ||
|-
|-
! Both
| kake || =(e)kak || Several days ago, last week || Example ||
| hani
| hanisi=
|-
|-
! Each
| nateha || =nate || The day before yesterday ||  Example ||
| nune
| nisi=
|-
|-
! Few
| kirá || =kira || Yesterday || Example ||
| cato
| catci=
|-
|-
! Another/Other
! rowspan="4" | Diurnal
| hane <!-- cf Minhast 'xani' -->
| yohori || =ho || Earlier this morning || Example ||
| hanesi=
|}
 
=== Interrogatives ===
 
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
|-
|-
! Header text
| tantori || =tora || Just awhile ago || Example ||
! Header text
! Notes
|-
|-
! Who
| yot || =(e)yót || Now || Example ||
| ra'
| Possibly cognate with Minhast ''redad''
|-
|-
! What
| nas || =(e)nas || Soon || Example ||
| pôh
| c.f. Minhast ''bak''
|-
|-
! When
! rowspan="3" | Future
| tu'a
| kesór || =k(e)sor || Tomorrow || Example ||
|
|-
|-
! Where
| nekós || =n(i)kos || In the next few days || Example ||
| nahí'
| c.f. Minhast ''nakki''. Also, note dialectal variant ''enket'' (Sapshira dialect)
|-
|-
! Why
| nankóy || =n(a)koy || In the indeterminate future || Example ||
| penóh
|
|-
|-
! Which
| yôri-
|
|}
|}


 
===Quantifiers===
The Interrogatives manifest WH-movement, appearing immediately before the auxiliary ''itá' '' with rising intonation and a slight pause after the main VP. 
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
 
! Quantifier
{{Gloss
! Substantive
|phrase =Okep nari ayrarke, penóh hô'itá'?
! Attributive
| IPA = 'okɛp 'na:ri aɪ'rarke pe'noɦ ɦo:ʔi'taʔ
|-
| morphemes = Okep nari ayrarke, penóh hô-'itá'
! All
| gloss = PN child spank why HOD-DIR-HASxLAS.COP
| kamun
| translation = Why did Okep spank (her) child earlier this morning?
| kamun si=
}}
|-
 
! Most
{{Gloss
| toman
|phrase =Ka tahone, ra taspitá'?
| toman si=
| IPA = ka ta'ɦo:ne ra taʃpi'taʔ
| morphemes = Ka ta-honé, ra  ta-sp-itá
| gloss = 2S INV-cry who INV-CAUS-HASxLAS.COP
| translation = Who is making you cry?
}}
 
 
All Interrogatives can also serve as  conjunctions,  eg ''Hôri ak kosno tā'itá' penóh Okep nari ayrarke ho'' (I did not hear/receive the news why Okep spanked (her) nephew early this morning").
 
Interrogatives can also serves as Conditionals by a ''=si'' Topicalization structure:
 
{{Gloss
|phrase = Ra'si karampo taneknek tā'itá, siakos tamakó itá'
| IPA = 'raʔʃi ka'rampo ta'nɛknek 'ta:ʔɪtaʔ 'ʃakoʃ tama'ko ɪ'taʔ
| morphemes = Ra'=si karampo ta=neknek tā='itá, si=akos tamakó itá'
| gloss = Who=CONN bear INV=kill INV=COP.SG CONN=1P danger.liability COP.SG
| translation = Whoever slaughtered the bear (in this manner) is a danger to us all.
}}
 
The Topic marker ''=si'' should not be confused with the Connective ''si='' that is linking the embedded clause to the matrix clause.  The former is an enclitic, whereas the latter is a proclitic.
 
Among the Interrogative pronouns, ''ra' '' ranks the highest in animacy, although it still is lower than personal pronouns.
 
''Yôri-'' is always prefixed to the noun it modifies and can never appear as an independent word. It can be prefixed to ''pôh'' and ''ra' '', i.e. ''yôripoh'' and ''yôra' '', where it takes the meaning "which one", ''yôripoh'' for Noun Classes II and III, the sentient members of Class I, and ''yôra''' for the rest.
 
=== Pronouns ===
The first group of pronouns are the simple independent pronouns.  The animacy of each pronoun is arranged in the following hierarchy:
 
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
|-
|-
! Pronominal hierarchy
! Some
| cene    <!-- cf Minhast 'ikyem' -->
| cisi=<br/> cis=
|-
|-
| style="text-align:center"|1st >> 2nd >> 3rd
! Many
|}
| eco
 
| ecisi=<br/> eci=<br/>ec=
Plurality generally does not affect the animacy level across persons.  However, if two 3rd person arguments have the same animacy and one of the arguments is plural, then the plural argument is assigned a higher animacy level.
|-
 
! Both
The following table lists the forms of the simple independent pronouns:
| hani
 
| hanisi=<br/>hansi=<br/>has=
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
|-
|+ '''Intransitive Independent  Pronouns'''
! Each
|-  
| nune
!
| nisi=<br/>nis=
! Singular
|-
!  Plural
! Few
| cato
| caci=<br/>cac=
|-
|-
! 1S
! Another/Other
| ak
| hane <!-- cf Minhast 'xani' -->
| akôs, akôsh
| hanesi=<br/>hanse=
|-  
! 2S
| kā, ka
| kakôs, kakôsh
|-  
! 3S
| tā, ta
| takôs, takôsh
|-
|}
|}


=== Interrogatives ===


If used as a core argument, they appear in intransitive clauses only, and just before the clause-final copula ''itá'',e.g. ''Nitori kā itá!'' (You are a fool); sometimes the pronoun is cliticized to the main verb, e.g.  ''Nitoriká' itá!'', in which case a final long vowel is shortened and is appended with the glottal stop.  They may also be used to emphasize a noun, in which case the pronoun precedes the noun linked with the connective ''si='', as in ''tā shi=naho'' (She/Her, the mother).
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
 
 
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
|+ '''Transitive Portmanteau Pronouns'''
|-
! colspan="4"| Singular
! colspan="4"| Plural
|-
|-
!
! Header text
! 1S
! Header text
! 2S
! Notes
! 3S
! 1P
! 2P
! 3P
|-
|-
! 1S
! Who
|  
| ra',
--
| Possibly cognate with Minhast ''redad''
| akak
| akat
|
--
| akakos, akakosh
| akatos, akatosh
|-
! 2S
| akak
|
--
| kat
| akoska, akoshka
|
--
| katakos, katakosh
|-
! 3S
| akat
| kat
| tat
| akosta, akoshta
| kakosta, kakoshta
| takosta, takoshta
|-
! colspan="8"| Plural x Plural
|-
|-
! colspan="4"| 1P
! What
|  
| pôh
--
| c.f. Minhast ''bak''
| akoskos, akoshkosh
|-
| akostos, akoshtosh
! When
|-  
| tu'a
! colspan="4"| 2P
| akoskos, akoshkosh
|-
|  
! Where
--
| nahí'
| kakostos, kakoshtosh
| c.f. Minhast ''nakki''. Also, note dialectal variant ''enket'' (Sapshira dialect)
|-  
|-
! colspan="4"| 3P
! Why
| akostos, akoshtosh
| penóh
| kakostos, kakoshtosh
|
| takostos, takoshtosh
|-
|-  
! Which
| yôri-
| C.f. ''yôhori'', to choose, Minhast ''yāhuran'' (to make a turn at an intersection of a path, road)
|-
! rowspan="2" | Polarity
| aní
| Used for yes-no questions, occurs in sentence-final position.<br/>
Cognate with Minhast ''ni'' (Classical, Horse Speaker, and Salmonic dialects)
|-
| =yo
| Like ''ani'', used for yes-no questions.  However, it cliticizes to the first word of the clause.
|}
|}




As earlier stated, nouns in Nankôre are not inflected, nor are case clitics attached to them to indicate case relationsTransitive verbs take two NPs as core arguments. The case relations of the two arguments are determined by the position of the noun in the animacy hierarchy; the core argument that is higher in the animacy hierarchy is assigned the Agent role, and the other argument is assigned the Patient role.  However, an inverse affix ''tā=/tāh=/ta=/t='' is prefixed to the primary verb, as well as a redundant ''ta-'' affix that is added to the auxiliary to alter the argument structure and promote the lower-animacy argument to Agent role.
The Interrogatives manifest WH-movement, appearing immediately before the auxiliary ''itá' '' with rising intonation and a slight pause after the main VP.  The Irrealis suffix ''-hi'' often accompanies WH-words but is often dropped, as in the second example, especially among younger speakers.  However, it is required for polar questions, i.e. questions marked with ''aní'', in which case its allmorph ''-ha'' is used.


Unlike the Intransitive forms, the portmanteau Transitive forms come before the main verb, not the auxiliary verb, e.g. ''Akostos karok tā'itá' '' , "I ate it".  Sometimes the portmanteau is cliticized to the main verb, as in  ''Akostos=karok tā'itá' ''.  If the Inverse marker appears simultaneously with the Transitive portmanteau pronoun, the preverbal Inverse marker ''tā=/tāh=/ta=/t='' cliticizes to the portmanteau pronoun.  Since the Inverse voice is double-marked, the Inverse form of the auxiliary ''itấ' '' also is used, as in the improbable sentence, ''Takostos karok tahortā'itá'/Takostoskarok tahortā'itá' '', from ''Tā-akostos-karok tahortā'itá' '', "He/she/it ate me".
{{Gloss
|phrase =Okep nari ayrarke, penóh hô'itáhi?
| IPA = 'okɛp 'na:ɾi aɪ'ɾaɾke pe'noɦ ɦo:ʔɪ'taɦi
| morphemes = Okep nari ayrarke, penóh Ø-hô-Ø-'itá-ʔ-hi
| gloss = PN child spank why DIR-HOD-HS-COP-LS-IRR
| translation = Why did Okep spank (her) child earlier this morning?
}}


Oblique arguments on the other hand are treated as adjuncts, and are thus bound to their clause with the connective clitic ''si=''.  Context alone determines the semantic role of the oblique.  Because obliques are considered adjuncts, they may not come between the core arguments and the verb, and therefore must appear either before or after the clause nucleus.  To illustrate, the sentence ''Maska si=ohipna koykare ekán itá'' (anthill=CONN twig boy twist COP.PST), i.e. "The boy inserted the twig into the anthill", the noun ''maska'' (anthill) is the oblique argument and is marked with the clitic ''si='' to join it to the rest of the sentence, e.g. ''Ohipna koykare ekán itá si=maska''.
{{Gloss
|phrase =Kā tahône, ra' tasip'itá'?
| IPA = ka: ta'ɦo:nɛ ɾaʔ taʃɪpʔ'ɪta
| morphemes = kā ta=hone, ra' ta-sip-Ø-itá
| gloss = 2S INV.SG=cry who INV-CAUS-HS-COP-LS
| translation = Who is making you cry?
}}


===Verbs===
The following two sentences are polarity questions, with no difference in meaning. The third sentence, containing both polarity markers ''=yo'' and sentence-final particle ''aní'', implies incredulity:


Verbs mark for [blah blah blah blah]
{{Gloss
|phrase = Koykâre cori yoskara sip'itáha' aní?
| IPA = /koɪ'ka:ɾɛ 'tʃoɾi 'joʃkaɾa ʃɪpʔɪtaɦa a'ni/
| morphemes = koykâre cori yoskara sip-Ø-itá-ʔ-hi aní
| gloss = boy house burn CAUS-DIR.PRS.HS-COP-LS-IRR Q
| translation = Did the boy burn the house (down)?
}}


====Copula Forms ====
{{Gloss
|phrase = Koykâryo cori yoskara sip'itáhi?
| IPA = /koɪ'ka:ɾjo 'tʃoɾi 'joʃkaɾa ʃɪpʔɪ'taɦi/
| morphemes = koykâre=yo cori yoskara sip-Ø-itá-ʔ-hi
| gloss = boy=Q house burn CAUS-DIR.PRS.HS-COP-LS-IRR
| translation = Did the boy burn the house (down)?
}}
 
{{Gloss
|phrase = Koykaryo cori yoskara  sip'itáha aní?
| IPA = /koɪ'kaɾjo 'tʃoɾi 'joʃkaɾa ʃɪpʔɪ'taɦa 'ani/
| morphemes = koykâre=yo cori yoskara sip-Ø-itá-ʔ-hi ani
| gloss = boy=Q house burn CAUS-DIR.PRS.HS-COP-LS-IRR Q
| translation = Did the boy burn the house (down)?
}}
 
With the exception of the polarity particle ''aní'', interrogatives can also serve as  conjunctions, provided that the interrogatives is joined to the dependent clause with the connective ''si='':
 
{{Gloss
|phrase = Horyák kosno nitá' penóh si-Okep nari ayrarkekór ho'itá'.
| IPA =
| morphemes = hôri=ák kosno Ø-n-Ø-itá-ʔ penóh si=Okep nari ayrarke-kór Ø-ho-Ø-'itá-ʔ
| gloss = NEG=1S hear  DIR-PST-HS.COP-LS why CONN=PN nephew spank-HEAR DIR-HOD-HS-COP-LS
| translation = I did not hear/receive the news why Okep spanked (her) nephew early this morning.
}}
 
Neither a pseudo-incorporated noun or adjunct may appear between the Interrogative and the auxiliary. The next sentence is well-formed, with the pseudo-incorporated noun appearing before the interrogative ''penóh'' (why), whereas the next sentence is unfelicitous, since PINs are barred from appearing between the WH-word and the auxiliary:
 
{{Gloss
|phrase = Koykâre yoskara cori penóh itá'?
| IPA =
| morphemes = koykâre yoskara cori penóh Ø-itá-ʔ
| gloss = boy burn house why DIR.PRS.HS-COP-LS
| translation = Why did the boy burn the house (down)?
}}


{| class="bluetable lightbluebg sortable mw-collapsible"
{{Gloss
!  rowspan="10"  style="width:64.01pt; " |  Present
|phrase = *Koykâre yoskara penóh cori sip'itá'?
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| IPA =  
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| morphemes = koykâre yoskara penóh cori sip-Ø-itá-ʔ
!  colspan="4" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
| gloss = boy house burn why CAUS-DIR.PRS.HS-COP-LS
|- 
| translation = Why did the boy burn the house (down)?
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
}}
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
 
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
Interrogatives can also serves as Conditionals by a ''=si'' Topicalization structure:
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
 
|-
{{Gloss
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|phrase = Ra'si karámpo taneknek ta'itá', siakos tamáko itá'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| IPA = 'raʔʃi ka'ɾampo ta'nɛknɛk 'taʔɪtaʔ 'ʃakoʃ ta'mako ʔɪ'taʔ
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| morphemes = Ra'=si karampo ta=neknek ta-Ø-itá-Ø, si=akos tamakó Ø-Ø-itá-ʔ
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| gloss = Who=TOP bear INV=kill INV-HS-COP-LS CONN=1P danger.liability DIR-HS-COP-LS
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| translation = Whoever slaughtered the bear (in this manner) is a danger to us all.
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
}}
|- 
 
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
The Topic marker ''=si'' should not be confused with the Connective ''si='' that is linking the embedded clause to the matrix clauseThe former is an enclitic, whereas the latter is a proclitic.  
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | itá'
Among the Interrogative pronouns, ''ra' '' ranks the highest in animacynevertheless, Interrogative pronouns are lower than non-Interrogative nouns in the animacy hierarchy.
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | itán
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | sip'itá' (pis'itá')
''Yôri-'' is always prefixed to the noun it modifies and can never appear as an independent word. It can be prefixed to ''pôh'' and ''ra' '', i.e. ''yôripoh'' and ''yôra''<nowiki>'</nowiki> where it takes the meaning "which one", ''yôripoh'' for Noun Classes II and III, the sentient members of Class I, and ''yôra''<nowiki>'</nowiki> for the rest.
| style="width:64.01pt; " | sip'itán  (pis'itán)
 
|-
<!--
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | nitá'
=== Demonstratives ===
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nitán
Attributive
| style="width:64.01pt; " | sipnitá' (pisnitá')
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | sipnitán (pisnitán)
 
|-
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
!
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! Pronominal !! Attributive<ref>The attributive forms derive from the pronominal + ''si-'' connective. Note that the connective cliticizes to the demonstrative, not the noun head as expected.</ref> !! Adverbial <ref>The adverbial ''-e'' suffix derives from a putative ''*e='' (c.f. Minhast ''eyha'' "there is/are") that cliticized to the pronominal form.</ref> !! Minhast Cognates
! colspan="4" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Patient
|-
|-
! Proximal
style="width:64.01pt; " |
|| hāpe || hapsi || hāpe, hape || sappu
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
! Medioproximal
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
|| nas || napsi || nāse, nase || naš
|-
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! Distal
style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|| yas || yapsi ||yāse, yase || wašia
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! Invisible
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|| ucce, ucca || ucci || ucce || suxta <!-- from *husta, c.f. Minhast suxta "thither", x(Minhast) vs h/0(Nan.), št(Min.) vs cc(Nan.)
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|}
|-
 
rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Agent
-->
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tā’itá
=== Pronouns ===
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tā’itán
Nankôre pronouns distinguish animacy, number, and transitivity.  The transitive forms are portmanteaus, which in combination with the presence or absence of the verb's inverse marker, encodes both the agent and patient. 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | taspitá
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | taspitán
Animacy in Nankôre is arranged in the following hierarchy:
|-
 
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tānitá
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tānitán
! Pronominal hierarchy
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnitá
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnitán
| style="text-align:center"|1st > 2nd > 3rd > 4th
|-
|}
!  rowspan="11" style="width:64.01pt; " | Hodiernal Past
 
|-
Plurality generally does not affect the animacy level across persons. However, if two 3rd person arguments have the same animacy and one of the arguments is plural, then the plural argument is assigned a higher animacy level.
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
The paradigm for the transitive pronouns are as follows:
! colspan="4" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
 
|-
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|+ '''Transitive Portmanteau Pronouns'''
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
! colspan="5"| Singular
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
! colspan="6"| Plural
|-
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
!
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! 1S !! 2S !! 3S !! 4S !! 1P.INC || 1P.EXCL !! 2P !! 3P || 4P
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! 1S
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| -- || ekká' || ekenko || enkár || -- || -- || kamme || enka ||nerá
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-  
|-
! 2S
! rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
| ekká' <br/> kā <br/> ka || --|| kanko <br/> kā <br/> ka || korí || kanná || kanné || -- || kanenko || kâre
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hô'itá
! 3S
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hô'itán
| ekenko|| kanko <br/> kā <br/> ka || -- || enkará || enko || nenko || kanenko || -- || kankór
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hô'ipnitá
|-  
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hô'ipnitán
! 4S
|-
| enkár || korí || enkará || -- ||  ánkare || nénkare || kánnare || eranka || --
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hô'intá
! colspan="11"| Plural x Plural
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hô'intán
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hô'ipnintá
! colspan="5"| 1P.INCL
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hô'ipnintán
| -- || -- || ánkame || ananka || ánnahi
|-
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! colspan="5"| 1P.EXCL
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| -- || -- || nékkame || nenka || énnahi
!  colspan="4" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
|-
|-
! colspan="5"|  2P
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| ánkame || nékkame || -- || nekákkame || nákkame
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
! colspan="5"| 3P
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
||ananka || nenka || nekákkame || -- || nékkahi
|-
|-  
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! colspan="5"| 4P
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|| ánnahi || énnahi|| nákkame || nékkahi || --
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-  
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|}
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
Transitive verbs take two NPs as core arguments. The case relations of the two arguments are determined by the position of the noun in the animacy hierarchy; the core argument that is higher in the animacy hierarchy is assigned the Agent role, and the other argument is assigned the Patient role.  However, an inverse affix ''tā=/tāh=/ta=/t='' is prefixed to the primary verb,  as well as a redundant ''ta-'' affix that is added to the auxiliary to alter the argument structure and promote the lower-animacy argument to Agent role.
|-
 
! rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Agent
The intransitive forms consist of both independent and cliticized forms, as illustrated in the following table:
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
| style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’hô'itá
|+ '''Intransitive Pronouns'''
| style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’hô'itán
|-  
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhópnitá
! rowspan ="2"| Person
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhópnitán
! colspan="2" | Singular
|-  
!  colspan="2" | Plural
style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhô'intá
! Independent !! Clitic !! Long Form !! Clitic
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhô'intán
|-  
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhópnintá
! 1
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhópnintán
| style="text-align:center"| eyak  || =yak <br/> =ak || aná || =na
|- 
|-
!  rowspan="11" style="width:64.01pt; " | Recent Past
! 2
|-  
| style="text-align:center"| eká  || =ka  || kamme || =kamme
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! 3
! colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
| style="text-align:center"| enko || =ko <br/>=na || enká || =enká <br/> =nka <br/> =ka
|-   
|-  
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! 4
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| style="text-align:center"| erá  || =ra || nami || =nami
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
|}
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
 
|
If used as a core argument, the intransitive singular forms and the plural '''short''' forms may appear just before the clause-final auxiliary ''itá'',e.g. ''Nitori kā 'itá!'' (You are a fool); or the clitic form of the pronoun attaches to the main verb, e.g. ''Nitoriká 'itá!''. They may also be used to emphasize a noun, in which case the pronoun precedes the noun linked with the connective ''si='', as in ''tā si=naho'' (She/Her, the mother).
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
As earlier stated, nouns in Nankôre are not inflected, and the same is true for pronouns.  Moreover, oblique pronominal arguments cannot be inserted into the verb phrase, but instead must appear before it. If oblique arguments appear outside their clause, particularly in topic-comment structures, they must be joined to it with the connective clitic ''si=''Context alone determines the semantic role of the oblique. To illustrate, the sentence ''Maska si=ohipna koykare ekán itá'' (anthill=CONN twig boy twist COP.PST), i.e. "The boy inserted the twig into the anthill", the oblique noun ''maska'' (anthill is marked with the clitic ''si='' to join it to the rest of the sentence.
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
 
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
===Verbs===
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
The verb phrase in Nankôre requires at a minimum an initial verb, plus one of the clause-final auxiliaries, '' 'itá' '' for transitive clauses or intransitive clauses where the subject's theta-role is that of Agent, and '' 'iná'' ' for all other intransitive clauses. A sort of division of labor exists between these components; one set of clitics or affixes may attach to one of the components, and another set to the other component.
|- 
 
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
The following set of markers cliticize to the first verb of the VP, which may be either the main verb, or a coverb. All preverbal clitics attach to the the initial verb:
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
#Inverse
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nitá
#Aspect
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nitán
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | sipnitá
However, a set of bound morphemes, occurring as suffixes, attach exclusively to the main verb, regardless of whether the verb occurs initial position in the verb phrase or not:
| style="width:64.01pt; " | sipnitán
 
|-
#Evidentials
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
{| class="wikitable"
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nintá
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nintán
! Evidentials !! Meaning !! Example
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | sipnintá
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | sipnintán
| -kor || hearsay || Example
|
|-
style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| Example || Example || Example
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-
! colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
| Example || Example || Example
|-
|}
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
Modal verbs, when they occur, follow the main verb, displaying leftward scope-ordering, e.g. ''Kôreak képorih iná'!'' "I am trying to speak!".
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
 
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
Finally, the auxiliary '' 'iná'/'itá' '' hosts the following set of affixes:  
|-
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
#Inverse
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
#Tense
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
#Number
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
#Voice
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
#Irrealis
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
|-
 
! rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Agent
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
====The Verb Phrase Structure ====
| style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’nitá
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’nitán
The following example demonstrates a complex verb predicate, with the Inverse marker ''ta='' cliticizing to the coverb ''yam'' (accompany) in the Imperfect aspect, followed by the main verb ''yayak'' (go up a mountain) with the Hearsay Evidential suffix ''-kor'', and finally the auxiliary marked with the redundent Inverse clitic ''ta'=''.
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnitá
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnitán
{{Gloss
|- 
|phrase = Rompóy kurasno tayampo yayak hosmakór ta'nitá'.
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| IPA = /rom'poɪ ku'raʃno ta'yampo 'yayak hoʃma'kor taʔnɪt'aʔ/
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tānintá
| morphemes = Rompóy kurasno ta-yam-po yayak hosma-kor ta'-∅-nitá-ʔ.
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tānintan
| gloss = dog.LA girl.HA INV-accompany-IMPF go.up.mountain-EVID.HEAR INV-HS-PST.COP-LS
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnintá
| translation = The dog was going up the mountain with the girl, I heard.
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnintán
}}
|-  
 
!  rowspan="11" style="width:64.01pt; " | Medio-Distal Past
The verbal elements follow a relatively strict order. The required elements for a VP to be well-formed are the Main Verb and the Auxiliary. Adjuncts are unbound morphemes, and exhibit rather free order; however, they are barred from appearing between the Inverse Marker and any following verb, be it the Coverb or the Main Verb.  If a Coverb appears between the Inverse Marker and Main Verb, an adjunct may appear between the Coverb and Main Verb.  However, if the Inverse Marker is not followed by a Coverb, then an adjunct may not appear between the Inverse Marker and the Main Verb.  Moreover, if Pseudo-Noun Incorporation (PNI) takes place, an adjunct may not intervene between the Main Verb and the Pseudo-incorporated noun (PIN).
|-
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
Although Nankôre is not a polysynthetic language, templatic representations like those of its polysynthetic cousins
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
Minhast and Nahónda, provide a useful visual tool to demonstrate the Nankôre verb phrase structure:
!  colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
 
|-
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|+ '''Verb Conjugation Table'''
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
! Position !! Type !! Subtype !! Realization
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
|-
|-
! -3
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| Inverse Marker 1
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| style="background-color:black"|
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| ta=  
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
! rowspan="3"| -2
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| rowspan="3"| Coverbs
|-
|-
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
| Directionals/Positionals ||
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hitá
| Modals ||
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hitán
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hispitá
! 1
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hisiptán
| Aspect
|-
| rowspan="3" style="background-color:black"|x
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| Examplexxx
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hintá
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hintán
! 0
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hisipnitá
| Primary Verb
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hisipnitán
|
|-   
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! +1
style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| Quasi-Incorporated Noun
! colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Patient
| Examples
|
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! +2
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| rowspan="4"| Auxiliary
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
| Inverse Marker 2
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
| ta-
|-
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! +3
style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| Inchoative
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! +4
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| Causative
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|
|-
|-
rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Agent
! +5
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| Irrealis
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhitá
|  
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhitan
|}
style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsiphitá
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsiphitán
====Coverbs====
|-
[https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Coverb Coverbs] in Nankôre formal grammar are auxiliary verbs that serve to provide additional syntactic information to the main verb itself, or to the core argumentsAlthough several types of coverbs exist, the Directionals-Positionals coverbs are the most prominentDirectional and positional coverbs serve as a way of indicating case relations of the core arguments, somewhat like Applicatives in other languages, such as those of its distant cousin [[Minhast]]. In fact the older linguistic literature often refers to these coverbs as "Applicative Verbs".  The Directionals-Positionals coverbs always precede all other modal verbs, which are scope-ordered.
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhintá
=====Directional-Positional Coverbs=====
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhintan
Directional and positional coverbs have both a long and short formThe long form, also called the Independent Form, may serve as a bona fide standalone verb, or it may serve as a coverb provided that no clitics attach to itThus the coverb ''sanha'', in its long form, means "to go towards, to approach" as an independent verb, but its short form, ''san'', always indicates motion towards an entity; this sort of syntactic relation is usually handled by the Allative case in languages which indicate case marking on the noun.  Clitics that otherwise attach to the main verb, such as the Inverse marker and and aspect markers, move to and attach to the first occurring coverb of the VP.  Clitics attach only to the short form.  Additionally, morphophonemic alternations may take place when the Inverse marker ''ta='' cliticizes to the coverb, such as in the Subessive ''cor-''.
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsiphintá
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsiphintán
 
|-   
{class="bluetable lightbluebg"  
!  rowspan="10" style="width:64.01pt; " | Distal Past
|+ style="font-weight:bold"| Goal Coverb Forms
|-
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
!  !! Long !! Short !! Coverb + Inverse
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-
! colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
! Goal
|
| sanha || san || tasan
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|}
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
 
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
{{Gloss
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|phrase = Anut America sanha kamukkor itáʔ.
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| IPA =
|-
| morphemes = anut america sanha kamuk-kor itáʔ
! rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
| gloss =PN.HA PN.LA going.towards fly-EVID.HEAR COP.DIR
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| translation = Anut supposedly flew to America.  
| style="width:64.01pt; " | pitá
}}
| style="width:64.01pt; " | pitán
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | pisiptá
{{Gloss
| style="width:64.01pt; " | pisiptan
|phrase = Rompóy sikatti yacor pitasi san yapokor itáʔ.
|-
| IPA =
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| morphemes = rompóy si=katti yacor san yapo-kor itáʔ
| style="width:64.01pt; " | pintá
| gloss = dog CONN=PN.HA ball.LA going.towards throw-EVID.HEAR COP.DIR
| style="width:64.01pt; " | pintán
| translation = Kathy tossed the ball towards the dog.  
| style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipnitá
}}
| style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipnitán
 
|-
{{Gloss
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|phrase = Katti yacor san yapokor rompóy itáʔ.
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| IPA =
!  colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Patient
| morphemes = Katti yacor san yapokor rompóy itáʔ
|-
| gloss = PN.HA ball.LA going.towards throw-EVID.HEAR dog COP.DIR
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| translation = Kathy tossed the ball towards the dog.  
style="width:64.01pt; " |  
}}
colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
 
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
{{Gloss
|-
|phrase = Kurasno siyacor rompóy tasan nahoyra ayorkor ta'itáʔ.
style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| IPA =
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| morphemes = Kurasno siyacor rompóy ta-san nahoyra ayor-kor ta-ʔitáʔ
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| gloss = girl CONN=ball.LA dog.HA INV-going.towards do.repeatedly bring-EVID.HEAR dog INV-COP
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| translation = The dog repeatedly brought back the ball to the girl.  
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
}}
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
|-
*Benefactive
! rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Agent
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’pitá
<!--
| style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’pitán
As an example, the auxiliary ''kohán'' passivizes the clause, resulting in a one-argument clause containing only the logical PatientSince there is only one core argument, the need for inverse marking no longer exists, and in fact the presence of both the passive auxiliary ''kohán'' and the inverse marker ''tā-/tāh-''' in the same sentence is ungrammatical. The same is true for the antipassive auxilliary ''norhe''/''norhâ'', that is the logical Patient is deleted leaving only the agent, and so once again, the presence of the inverse ''tā-/tāh-'' is ungrammaticalWhen either the Agent or the Patient has been deleted by the passive or antipassive auxiliaries, one thing that must be noted is that the demoted arguments are fully deleted. In other languages that have passives and/or antipassives, demoted core arguments need not be deleted, in fact in many languages the demoted argument may still be retained in the sentence, but this time as an oblique argument. Such is not the case in Nankôre; the former core argument cannot appear in the sentence.
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tapsipta
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tapsiptan
The applicative verbs promote an Oblique argument to core status, thereby creating a derived Patient. This results in a transitive sentence, and the inverse marker ''tā-/tāh-'' once again can be used to disambiguate the roles of the core arguments if a lower-animacy argument is an Agent.  Interestingly, unlike passivation and antipassivation, the demoted core argument, which is always the former Patient, can still appear in the clause as an Oblique argument, marked with the connective ''si-''  There are several of these auxiliaries, listed in the following table:
|-
-->
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tapintá
{|  class="bluetable lightbluebg mw-collapsible"  
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tápintan
|+ style="font-weight:bold"| Directional and Positional  Coverbs
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tapsipnitá
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tapsipnitán
! !! Long !! Short !! Coverb + Inverse !! Sample Sentence !! Translation !! Comment
|-
|-
!  rowspan="11" style="width:64.01pt; " | Medio-Proximal Future
! Goal
|-   
| sanha || san || tasan || Anut America sanha kamuk-kor itá. || Anut supposedly flew to America. ||  ''-kor'' = HEARSAY.EVID, is suffixed to the main verb.
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! Dative
! colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
| tohta || toht || tatóh || Anut nanna tatóh ittáh tanno ta'itá. || Anut gave his mother water. || c.f Minhast dative applicative ''-dut-'', ''menā''' "mother", ''hittu'' "give", ''dannua'' "water"
|-
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! Benefactive
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| nasko|| nas || tanas || Nanhoska karen tā-nas eynakor itá. || It is said the (sacred) tree gave (life) for the people.|| c.f. Minhast postposition ''=nī, =ni''
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
|-
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
! Comitative
|-
| yampe || yam || tam || Anut yam temenkor itá || Anut walked beside the river with him.|| ''temen'' = to perform an activity by a river
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! Instrumental
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| makôr || mak || tamak || Ehok mak-nahoyra yurasnanetkor itá. || The adolescent struck (it) repeatedly with a club.|| <br/>
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
! Ablative
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| risa || ris || tars || Šôni Nan Kamun Koryas ris mankor itá || First Man emerged from (out of) the snow. || c.f. Minhast applicative ''-raħk-''<br/> ''Šôni Nan'' = "the First Man" <br/>''Kamun Koryas'' "All Snow" (the birthplace of First Man)
|-
|-
! rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
! Locative
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| neyhi || ne || tan || Yonka asrok ta-n akunkor hô'itá. || I heard that a fly buzzed inside Yonka('s mouth) this morning.|| ''asrok'' = fly, insect
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nitáh
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nitáhan
! Vialis
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nisiptáh
| nahke || nak, nahk || tanak, tanahk || Ka pohak ta-nahke šokor-nui itá' || I saw the river take you through the cave. || c.f. Minhast applicative ''-naħk-, -ni-''<br/> ''pohak'' = cave <br/>''šokor'' = to flow<br/> ''-nui-'' = VIS.EVID
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nisiptahan
|-
|-
! Prolative
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| roskar || ros || taros|| Ak piri roskar hô'itá'. || I crossed the street earlier this morning || ''piri'' = street, road, path
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nintáh
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nintáhan
! Superessive
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | nisipnitáh
| oros || or || tor || Ayôhe arupenrô or yaunkekor hô'itá.|| The airplane flew over Ayôhe township earlier this morning.|| ''arupenrô'' = airplane <br/> ''yaunke'' = to fly
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | nisipnitáhan
|-
|- 
! Subessive
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| yorha || yor || toyor, cor || Ekurki saska yor-ro kahorokor itá. || The turtle swam underneath the ice.|| ''ekurki'' = turtle <br/>''saska'' = ice<br/>''kahoro'' = to swim
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-
!  colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Patient
! Ablative-Superessive
|-
| isuk || suk, su || task, tasku || (Example)|| (Example)||
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-  
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! Ablative-Subessive
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
| royna || roy || taroy || (Example)|| (Example)||
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
|-  
|-  
! Riverine
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| temen|| tem || tatem || No si-kurasno tem rohnaskor itá.|| The girls supposedly were playing by the river.||
| style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-  
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
! Montaigne
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| yayak|| yay || tay || Kahno côri yay cire/ciri itá' || The old man went up the mountain.||
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|}
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
|- 
====Aspect====
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Agent
The verb, in contrast to the auxiliary, encodes aspect with clitics that attach to the end of the word. If a coverb appears, the aspect marker will cliticize to the coverb, otherwise it will cliticize to the main verb.  Nankôre possesses several aspect markers, as illustrated in the following table:
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’nitáh
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"  
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tantáhan
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnitáh
! !! Aspect  Marker!Meaning
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnitáhan
|-
|- 
! style=""| Perfect
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| -Ø||
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tanintáh
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tanintáhan
! style=""| Semelfactive
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnitáh
| =no|| completed in one single motion/act, e.g. hit
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnintáhan
|-
|- 
! style=""| Simple Imperfect
!  rowspan="11" style="width:64.01pt; " | Immediate Future
| =ro||
|- 
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! style=""| Durative/Repetitive
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| =nahoyra, =noyra|| several times
!  colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
|-
|- 
! style=""| Habitative
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| =(h)osma, =sma|| often
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
! style=""| Factive
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
| =yosir|| (general truth statement, e.g. "The sun rises in the east")
|- 
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! style=""| Continuative
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
| =namporo|| to continue
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! style=""| Cessative
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| =ciri|| to stop
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
|- 
! style=""| Terminative
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
| =ruykáno|| to finish
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hitáh
! style=""| Incipient
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hitáhan
| =kara|| about to
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hisipitáh
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hisiptáhan
! style=""| Approximative
|- 
| =tontoro|| almost, nearly
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hintáh
! style=""| Frequentative
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hintáhan
| =nokori, -(o)nkori|| often
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hispintáh
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hisipnitáhan
|}
|- 
 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
====Main Verb ====
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
The main verb is believed to be descended from a verbal noun and carries the main semantic meaning of the verb phrase. The main verb hosts the Inverse Marker ''tā-/ta-'', aspect, and evidential/modal markers. As mentioned earlier, the Inverse and aspect markers may detach from the main verb and attach to a converb, while the modal/evidential suffixes remain tightly bound to the main verb.
!  colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Patient
 
|- 
===== Moods and Evidentials=====
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
The Mood and Evidential markers occur as bound suffixes of the main verb. Unlike the Inverse and aspect markers, they do not detach from the main verb but remain bound to the main verb.
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
 
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"  
|- 
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! colspan="2"|Evidentials
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
! style=""| Neutral
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| -
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! style=""| Emphatic-Factual
|- 
| -mas <br/> -masne
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Agent
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
! style=""| Hearsay
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhitáh
| -kor
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhitáhan
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhisipitáh
! style=""| Visual
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhisiptáhan
| -nui
|- 
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! style=""| Inferential
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhintáh
| -yay
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhintáhan
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhispintáh
! style=""| Intentive
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhisipnitáhan
| -neat, -ne
|- 
|-
!  rowspan="12" style="width:64.01pt; " | Distal Future
! style=""| Dubitative
|- 
| -anene
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|}
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
 
!  colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
=====Modals=====
|- 
 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
Modal verbs follow the main verb. Modals include a wide range of expressions, such as ability, potentiality, similitude, desire, inclination, etc.
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
 
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"  
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
|-
|- 
! !! Modal!!  Meaning !! Nahenic Cognates
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! style=""| Potential
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| makán|| be able to, can
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! style=""| Necessitive
|- 
| nehái|| need to
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
|
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pitáh
! style=""| Desiderative
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pitáhan
| tasmi|| want to
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pisiptáh
|
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pisiptáhan
|-
|- 
! style=""| Conative
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| si <br/> s=|| to try to, attempt
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pintáh
| Cognate with Minhast ''sunep-'' (to push)
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pintáhan
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipnitáh
! style=""| Inceptive
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipnitáhan
| yári|| begin to
|- 
|
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
! style=""| Resumptive
!  colspan="4" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Patient
| kenóy|| again
|- 
|
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|}
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
 
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
Examples of usage of the modal verbs follow:
|- 
 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
{{Gloss
|  style="width:64.01pt; " |  
|phrase = Ekenko tuas sitá', yankór<ref>From reconstructed P-Nahenic *''wa=man=kor'' (CONN=SEQ=and.yet).</ref> seskoro sioykar ináka.        
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| IPA = Ekenko tuaʃ 'ʃi'taʔ yan'koɾ 'ʃɛʃkoɾo 'ʃoɪkaɾ ɪ'naka
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| morphemes = ekenko tuas s-itá' yankór se=skoro sioykar iná-ka.  
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| gloss = 1S.3S wash.out CON-AUX.TRN however reed=DEM.PROX good be-NEG.
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| translation = I'm trying to wash (the stain) out, but this reed brush isn't good.
|- 
}}
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Agent
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
====The Auxiliary ====
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’pitáh
 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | taptahán
In terms of morphological complexity, the auxiliary hosts more affixes and clitics than the main verb; in addition to redundantly marking the Inverse voice, it also encodes tense, number, irrealis, inchoativity, and causation through a complex series of affixation.   
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tapsiptáh
 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tapsiptáhan
The auxiliary serves two purposes:
|- 
* To links noun phrases to their predicates:
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāpintáh
{{Gloss
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāpintáhan
|phrase = Nan konkekma yoriká ináhi?
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tapsipnitáh
| IPA = /nan kon'kekma 'yo:riká ɪn'ahi/
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tapsipnitáhan
| morphemes = nan konkekma, yôri-ká ∅-iná-ʔ-hi
|}
| gloss = human snake which-2S DIR-PRS.COP.STAT-S-IRR
| translation = Are you man or snake?
}}


====The Verb Phrase====
* To serve as a host for tense, number, animacy, and voice markers to the verb phrase.


The following example demonstrates a complex verb predicate, with the Inverse marker ''ta='' cliticizing to the coverb ''yam'' (accompany) in the Imperfect aspect, followed by the main verb ''yayak'' (go up a mountain) with the Hearsay Evidential suffix ''-kor'', and finally the copular verb marked with the secondary Inverse clitic ''tā=''.
{{Gloss
|phrase = Kanro kacekák  hôsipnitá'.
| IPA = /'kanro ka't͡ʃekák ho:sipnɪ'taʔ/
| morphemes = kanro kacek=ák ∅-hô-sip-nitá-ʔ
| gloss = old.man hide=1S DIR-HOD-HS-PST.COP.TRN-LS
| translation =I hid the old man yesterday.
}}


{{Gloss
{{Gloss
|phrase = Rompóy kurasno tayampo yayak hosmakor tā'itá'.
|phrase = Ea? Kacek ka, yôrikani hôsipnitáhi?
| IPA = /rom'poɪ ku'raʃno ta'yampo 'yayak hoʃma'kor ta:ʔɪt'/
| IPA = /ea 'kat͡ʃek ka, yo:ri'kani ho:ʃɪpni'tahi/
| morphemes = Rompóy kurasno ta=yam=po yayak hosma-kor tā'-itá'.
| morphemes =ea kacek ka yôri-kani  ∅-hô-sip-nitá-ʔ-hi
| gloss = dog girl INV=accompany=IMPF go.up.mountain-EVID.HEAR INV-PST.COP
| gloss = MIR hide 2S which-place DIR-HOD-CAUS-HS-PST.COP.TR-LS-IRR
| translation = The dog was going up the mountain with the girl, I heard.
| translation =Really?  Where did you hide him?
}}
}}


[[File:Nankore VP.png|thumb|Diagram of the Nankôre Verb Phrase]]
The Inverse marker ''ta'-'' indicates a lower animate NP is the Agent acting on a higher animate NP whose role is that of Patient.  The marker occurs in two locations: as a clitic to the first verb of the VP, and as a bound prefix to the auxiliary ''itá''':
The verbal elements follow a relatively strict order.  The required elements for a VP to be well-formed are the Main Verb and the Copula.  Adjuncts are unbound morphemes, and exhibit rather free order; however, they are barred from appearing between the Inverse Marker and any following verb, be it the Coverb or the Main Verb.  If a Coverb appears between the Inverse Marker and Main Verb, an adjunct may appear between the Coverb and Main Verb.  However, if the Inverse Marker is not followed by a Coverb, then an adjunct may not appear between the Inverse Marker and the Main Verb.  Moreover, if Quasi-Noun Incorporation (QNI) takes place, an adjunct may not intervene between the Main Verb and the quasi-incorporated noun (QIN), although bound adverbial  affixes may.


====Main Verb ====
{{Gloss
The main verb is believed to be descended from a verbal noun.  The main verb hosts the Inverse Marker ''tā-/ta-'', applicative, aspect, and evidential/modal markers. If a pronoun is used, it may cliticize to the first element of the verb.  However, one or more adjuncts may be inserted between the portmanteau pronoun and the verb, in which case the portmanteau cliticizes to the adjunct following it.
|phrase = kattí <u>ta</u>rayrokor <u>ta</u>'itá'.
| IPA = /ka't:i ta'raɪrokor taʔɪ'taʔ/
| morphemes = kattí ta-rayro-kor ta-itá-ʔ
| gloss = 2S+3S INV-play-HEAR INV.LS-PST-COP-LS
| translation = I heard that he played with you recently.
}}


<!--
Number is marked on both core arguments: for transitive clauses, singular HA arguments are null-marked, and LA arguments are marked with the suffix ''-ʔ''.  For plurality, plural HA arguments are marked with a prefixed or infixed ''-n-'', while the LA argument is marked with the suffix ''-n'' for plurality.  Additionally, the vowel of the Inverse marker lengthens when the LA argument is plural; for example, the singular LA argument of the present tense auxiliary is ''ta'ita<nowiki>'</nowiki>'', whereas the plural form of the verb is ''t<u>ā</u>'inta'''. The Hodiernal tense is an exception: the vowel of the Inverse marker neither undergoes vowel lengthening, nor does it receive compensatory stress; the form *''tāhô'intá<nowiki>'</nowiki>'' is ungrammatical, the form ''tahô'intá<nowiki>'</nowiki>'' instead. This is presumably because the vowel of the tense marker, ''-ô-'', suppresses lengthening of a preceding vowel according to Nankôre phonological rules, thus moving the stress rightward.
portmanteau=[INV-APP-ASP-EVID-MAIN.VERB] [AUX.VERB]
-->


The Inchoative consists of a the prefix ''yis-'', which frequently occurs as a circumfix, ''y''- + ''-s-'', for certain conjugations.  The affix causes the Inverse affix ''ta(h)-'' to palatize; the ''-s-'' segment of the circumfixal form may occur before or after a pluralizing ''-n-'' affix, or the tense-bearing prefix of a given conjugation.  Knowing which form of the Inchoative, and where the ''-s-'' segment occurs is unpredictable and requires memorization.  When used in transitive sentences, it serves to mark the Inceptive aspect.  The ''-s-'' component has been shown to be cognate with the Minhast inchoative affix ''-saxt-''.


=====Applicatives =====
The Causative is marked with the affixes ''sip-'' or ''-sp-'', which is cognate with the Causatives in Minhast ''-šp-'' and Nahónda ''-ps-''. As Causatives typically increase the valency in a clause's argument structure, the Causee is typically the Patient argument, and the target of the Causee is a ''si-'' marked argument phrase that follows the matrix clause; a slight pause usually occurs before the ''si-'' connective.  The construction suggest the Causee's target is an anti-topic, although it may be fronted at the beginning of its governing clause by the ''si-'' connective for topicalization.


The Applicative affixes betray their origins as motion and positional verbs. However, Applicatives, unlike their locomotive verbal counterparts, also alter the argument structure of a clause by promoting an oblique NP to a derived Patient argument; this promotion of an oblique argument triggers the demotion of the original Patient argument to oblique status.  Applicatives are mutually incompatible with the Passive and Antipassive markers that are carried on the auxiliary verb (see section on Auxiliary Verbs below)However, since the Applicatives increase the transitivity of a clause, thereby creating a derived Patient, the Inverse Prefix can be prefixed to the Applicative in the usual manner, namely, to mark lower animacy core arguments as Agents and higher animacy arguments as Patients in transitive clauses.  When the Inverse marker cliticizes to an Applicative, morphophonemic may alternations occur.
The Irrealis ''-hi'' is suffixed to the auxiliary.  It is obligatory for all Future tenses. It also surfaces in imperatives, yes-no questions which are marked with the ''=yo'' marker cliticizing to the first or second word of a sentence, as in ''Cire=yo tā'itá'-hi?'' "Did he die?", and in WH-questions when the event actually happening has not yet been established as having occurred; for example, ''Enket cire tā'itá'?'', "Where did he die?" implies that the person's death is a fact, and it is only the location that is being inquired, versus ''Enket cire tā'itá'-hi?'' implies that not only is the location unknown, but the person's actual death happening still remains to be establishedAn interesting structure, called the "double interrogative", is a combination of a WH-word followed by a word bearing the ''=yo'' clitic, which also turns the question into yes-no question: ''Enket cire=yo tā'itá'-hi?'', "Did he die, and if so, where?"  The ''-hi'' suffix is believed to be cognate with the Minhast Irrealis clitic ''=š''.


Because of their origin from motion and positional verbs, these affixes are prefixed to the main verb.
=====Transitive Paradigm =====


{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"  
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg sortable mw-collapsible"
|-
rowspan="10" style="width:64.01pt; " |  Present
! !! Applicative Marker!Inverse Marker + Applicative Marker!! Meaning
| colspan="2" rowspan="3" style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |  
|-
! colspan="6" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
! style=""| Dative/Allative
|-
| san-
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
| tasan-
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
| to, towards
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
|-
|-
! style=""| Benefactive
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| nas-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| tanas-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| for
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
! style=""| Commitative
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| yam-
|-
| tayam-, teym-, tem-
! rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
| with, along
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | itá'
! style=""| Instrumental
| style="width:64.01pt; " | itá'ita'
| mak-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yistá'
| tamak-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yistá'ita
| by means of, using
| style="width:64.01pt; " | sip'itá' (pis'itá')
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | sip'itá'ita  (pis'itá'ita')
! style=""| Ablative
|-
| ris-
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| tarsh-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nitá'
| from
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nitá'ita'
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitá'
! style=""| Locative
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitá'ita'
| ne-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | sipnitá' (pisnitá')
| tan-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | sipnitá'ita' (pisnitá'ita')
| in, within
|-
|-
| colspan="2" rowspan="3"  style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |
! style=""| Vialis
!  colspan="6" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Patient
| nak-
|-
| tanak-, tank-
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
| through
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
|-
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
 
|
! style=""| Prolative
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| rosh-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| torsh-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| across
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
! style=""| Elative
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| nakri-
|-
| takri-
! rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Agent
| out of
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’itá'
! style=""| Illative
| style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’itá'ita
| nak-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | castá'
| tanak-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | castá'ita
| into
| style="width:64.01pt; " | taspitá'
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | taspitá'ita
! style=""| Superessive
|-
| or-
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| tor-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tānitá' 
| above
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tānitá'ita
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | casnitá'
! style=""| Subessive
| style="width:64.01pt; " | casnitá'ita
| yor-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnitá'
| chor- (Shapshira dialect: "toyor-")
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnitá'ita
| below
|-  
|-
! rowspan="11" style="width:64.01pt; " | Hodiernal Past ‡
! style=""| Ablative-Superessive
|
| suk-
| colspan="2" rowspan="3"  style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |  
| tushk-, toshk-
! colspan="6" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
| from above
|-
|-
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
! style=""| Ablative-Subessive
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
| roy-
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
| toroy-
|-
| from below
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! style=""| Perlative
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| tek-
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| tatek-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| across
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
|-
! style=""| Riverine
! rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
| tem-, ten-
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| tatem, taten-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hô'itá'
| along a river
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hô'itá'ita
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yôsitá'
! style=""| Montaigne
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yôsitá'ita
| yayk-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hôsipnitá'
| teyk-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hôsipnitá'ita
| upwards on a mountain
|- 
|}
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hô'intá'
===== Aspect =====
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hô'intá'ita
 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yôsintá'
Nankôre has a wealth of aspect markers. These are clitics that attach to the first verb of the verb phraseThe Perfective Aspect is null-marked, but several non-Perfect Aspects are recognized:
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yôsintá'ita
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hôsipnintá'
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hôsipnintá'ita
 
|-
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"  
| colspan="2" rowspan="3"  style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |
|-
! colspan="6" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Patient
! !! Aspect  Marker!Meaning
|- 
|-
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
! style=""| Perfect
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
| -Ø||
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
|-
|-   
! style=""| Semelfective
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| =no|| completed in one single motion/act, e.g. hit
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
! style=""| Simple Imperfect
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| =po||  
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! style=""| Durative/Repetitive
|-
| =nahoyra, =noyra|| several times
! rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Agent
|-
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
! style=""| Habitative
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tahô'itá'
| =(h)osma, =sma|| often
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tahô'itá'ita
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | costá'
! style=""| Factive
| style="width:64.01pt; " | costá'ita
| =yosir|| (general truth statement, e.g. "The sun rises in the east")
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhópsintá'
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhópsintá'ita
! style=""| Continuative
|-
| =namporo|| to continue
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tahô'intá'
! style=""| Inceptive
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tahô'intá'ita
| =otah|| to begin
| style="width:64.01pt; " | cosnitá'
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | cosnitá'ita
! style=""| Cessative
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhópsintá'
| =ciri|| to stop
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhópsintá'ita
|-
|-
! style=""| Terminative
!  rowspan="11" style="width:64.01pt; " | Past
| =ruykáno|| to finish
|-
|-
| colspan="2" rowspan="3"  style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |  
! style=""| Incipient
!  colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
| =kara|| about to
|-
|-
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
! style=""| Approximative
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
| =tontoro|| almost, nearly
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
|-
|-
! style=""| Frequentative
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| =nokori, -(o)nkori|| often
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|}
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
===== Modals and Evidentials=====
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
|-
The Modal and Evidential suffixes occupy the final position of the main verb's template.
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"  
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nitá'
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nitá'ita
! colspan="2"|Mode
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitá'
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitá'ita
! style=""| Factual
| style="width:64.01pt; " | sipnitá'
| -mash
| style="width:64.01pt; " | sipnitá'ita
|-
|-
! style=""| Hearsay
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| -kor
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nintá'
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nintá'ita
! style=""| Visual
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitá'
| -nui
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitá'ita
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | sipnintá'
! style=""| Intentive
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | sipnintá'ita
| -neat
|-   
|}
|  colspan="2" rowspan="3" style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |
 
! colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Patient
<!--
|-  
https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Evidentiality
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
-->
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
 
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
====Auxiliary Verbs ====
|- 
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
Auxiliary verbs encode number, voice and tense. Intransitive pronominal core arguments, if used as independent pronouns, precede the auxiliary.
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
((More stuff to follow))
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
=====Animacy-Number Agreement Marking=====
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
|-
Transitive verbs mark number on core arguments by ablaut, or by adding a prefix, to the auxiliary. These markers indicate the number of both the higher animate and the lower animate arguments.
rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Agent
 
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"  
| style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’nitá'
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’nitá'ita
! !! Higher Animate Singular !! Higher Animate Plural
style="width:64.01pt; " | casnitá'
|-
style="width:64.01pt; " | casnitá'ita
! style=""| Lower Animate Singular
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tasipnitá'
| --
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tasipnitá'ita
| t(i)-
|
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! style=""| Lower Animate Plural
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tānintá'
| first /a/ lengthened to /a:/
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tānintá'ita
| ita-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | casnintá'
|}
| style="width:64.01pt; " | casnintá'ita
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnintá'
Unmarked:
style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnintá'ita
# Rompóy kurasno rayro ta'itá'. "The girl played with the dog."
|
# Rompóy kurasno tarayro ta'itá'. "The dog played with the girls."
!  rowspan="11" style="width:64.01pt; " | Medio-Distal Past
 
|
Higher Animate Plural x Lower Animate Singular
colspan="2" rowspan="3"  style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |  
# Rompóy kurasno rayro ti-ta'itá'. "The girls played with the dog."
!  colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
# Rompóy kurasno tarayro ti-ta'itá'. "The dog played with the girl."
|
 
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
Higher Animate Singular x Lower Animate Plural
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
# Rompóy kurasno rayro kor tā'itá'. "The girl played with the dogs."
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
# Rompóy kurasno tarayro kor tā'itá'. "The dogs played with the girl."
|
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
Higher Animate Plural x Lower Animate Plural
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
# Rompóy kurasno rayro ita-tā'itá'. "The girls played with the dogs."
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
# Rompóy kurasno tarayro ita-tā'itá'. "The dogs played with the girls."
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
=====Voice and Tense=====
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
|
 
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hitá'
! rowspan="2" |Tense-Aspect !! colspan="2" | Number
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hitá'ita
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yistá'
! Example | Singular || Plural
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yistá'ita
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hispitá'
| Example || Example || Example
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hisiptá'ita
|-
|-
| Example || Example || Example
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hintá'
| Example || Example || Example
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hintá'ita
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yisintá'
| Example || Example || Example
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yisintá'ita
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hisipnitá'
| Example || Example || Example
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hisipnitá'ita
|-
|
| Example || Example || Example
colspan="2" rowspan="3"  style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |  
|-
!  colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Patient
| Example || Example || Example
|
|-
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
| Example || Example || Example
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
|-
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
| Example || Example || Example
|-  
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| Example || Example || Example
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| Example || Example || Example
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| Example || Example || Example
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
|-
| Example || Example || Example
! rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Agent
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| Example || Example || Example
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tahitá'
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tahitá'ita
| Example || Example || Example
| style="width:64.01pt; " | castá'
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | castá'ita
| Example || Example || Example
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tasiphitá'
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tasiphitá'ita
| Example || Example || Example
|-
|-
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| Example || Example || Example
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhintá'
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhintá'ita
| Example || Example || Example
| style="width:64.01pt; " | casintá'
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | casinta'ita
| Example || Example || Example
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsiphintá'
|}
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsiphintá'ita
 
|
 
!  rowspan="11" style="width:64.01pt; " | Distal Past
 
|
 
| colspan="2" rowspan="3"  style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |  
The Nankôre verb is rich in voice and tense distinctionsThese distinctions are combined in an auxiliary verb, formed by adding one or more prefixes to the copular verb ''itá'' /ɪ'taʔ/, hence this auxiliary is called the ''itá''-verb. The ''itá''-auxiliary verb is always clause final, the main verb and any other VP particles preceding itSome voices also encode formality, with the long forms, e.g. ''man'itá', suphitá', tā'itá', hô'itá', and pā'itá' '', used for formal situations, and the short forms ''manta'/nitá', supta', tayta', hoyta''', and ''pitáh'' for informal conversation among friends and family.
!  colspan="6" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
 
|- 
Note that the Present Inverse form ''tā'itá''' is identical in form with that of the Past Direct form. Fortunately, the Inverse Voice is double-marked: the enclitic ''ta='' and its allomorphs ''tā=/tah=/tāh='' cliticizes to the main verb so no ambiguity arises.
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
 
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
 
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
HAS = High Animate Singular
|- 
 
style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
HAP = High Animate Plural
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
LAS = Low Animate Singular
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
LAP = Low Animate Plural
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
|- 
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"  
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
! !!colspan='4'| Present !! colspan='4'|Mythological Past !! colspan='4'| Distal Past !! colspan='4'|Past!! colspan='4'|Recent Morning Past !! colspan='4'| Future
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pitá'              
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | pitá'ita
!  !! HAS x LAS !! HAS x LAP || HAP x LAS ||HAP x LAP|| HAS x LAS ||HAS x LAP ||  HAP x LAS ||HAP x LAP || HAS x LAS ||HAS x LAP ||  HAP x LAS ||HAP x LAP|| HAS/LAS ||HAS x LAP ||  HAP x LAS ||HAP x LAP|| HAS x LAS ||HAS x LAP ||  HAP x LAS ||HAP x LAP|| HAS x LAS ||HAS x LAP ||  HAP x LAS ||HAP x LAP
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsitá'
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsitá'ita
! style="" | Direct
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pisiptá'
| itá'|| itâ' || titá' ||titâ' ||man'itá', manta', nitá' || man'itâ', mantâ', nitâ' || timan'itá', timanta', tintá' || timan'itâ', timantâ', tintâ' |suphitá', supta'||suphitâ', suptâ'|| tisuphitá', cupta' || tisuphitâ', cuptâ' || tā'itá', tayta' || tā'itâ', taytâ' || titā'itá', titayta' || titā'itâ', titaytâ' || hô'itá', hoyta' || hô'itâ', hoytâ' || tihô'itá', tihoyta' ||tihô'itâ', tihoytâ' || pā'itáhi, pitáh ||  pā'itâhi, pitâh ||  tipā'itáhi, tiptáh ||  tipā'itâhi, tiptâh
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipta'ita
|-
|-   
! style=""| Inverse
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| tā'itá', tayta'||x||x||x|| taman'itá', tamanta'||x||x||x|| tahorish'itá', taroshta'||x||x||x|| tahortā'itá', tahorta'||x||x||x|| tahorô'itá', taróyta' || x||x||x|| tahorpā'itáhi, torpáytah||x||x||x
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pintá'
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pintá'ita
! style=""| Simple Causative
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsintá'
| shiphitá'||x||x||x|| mashpitá' ||x||x||x ||  ishpitá'||x||x||x|| tashpitá'||x||x||x|| hoshpitá'||x||x||x|| pashpitáhi, pashpitah||x||x||x
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsintá'ita
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipnitá
! style=""| Double Causative
| style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipnitá'ita
| shipshiphitá'|| mashishpitá'|ishishpitá'|| tashishpitá'|| hoshishpitá'|| pashishpitáhi, pashishpitah
|-   
|-
|  colspan="2" rowspan="3"  style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |
! style=""| Intransitive Active
!  colspan="6" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Patient
| kitá'|| mankitá'|| ishkitá'|| tākitá', takta'|| hôkitá', hokta' || pākitáhi, paktah
|-
|-
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
! style=""| Intransitive Stative
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
| itá'|| mankitá'|| ishkitá'|| tākitá', takta'|| hôkitá', hokta' || pākitáhi, paktah
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
|-
|-  
! style=""| Passive
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| horkitá'|| horankitá'|| horishkitá'|| hortākitá', hortakta|| horôkitá', horokta || horpākitáhi, horpaktah
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
! style=""| Reflexive
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| shorkitá' || mashorkitá'|| ishorkitá'|| tashorkitá'|| hoshorkitá' || pashorkitáhi, pashorkitáh
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! style=""| Reciprocal
|- 
| soshorkitá' || masoshorkitá'|| isoshorkitá'|| tasoshorkitá'|| hososhorkitá' || pasoshorkitáhi, pasoshorkitah
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Agent
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
! style=""| Middle/Inchoative
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’pitá'
| hankitá'|| mankitá'|| yankitá'|| tankitá'|| honkitá'||pankitáhi
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’pitá'ita
|}
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | caspitá
 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | casipnitá'ita
<br/>
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tápsipnitá'
Nankôre has five intransitive voices, two basic Intransitive voices, one for Active verbs and another for Stative verbs; a Passive-Direct, a Reflexive, and a Reciprocal. A submorpheme ''-k-'' is found in all four intransitive voices, although it is lacking in the Present Tense form of the Intransitive Stative FormThe Active and Stative Intransitive forms are otherwise identical.
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tápsipnitá'ita
 
|-  
The Passive demotes a '''lower animate''' core argument, which is then relegated to oblique status or is dropped from the clause.  All Intransitive voices are incompatible with the Inverse marker ''ta=/tā=/tah=/tāh='' that cliticizes to the main verb.
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tápsintá'
<!--
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tápsintá'ita
Detransitive Voices I and II remove one of the core arguments in a transitive clause, thus changing its argument structure by decreasing its valency. The Detransitive I voice removes the core NP argument that has the higher animacy level, and the Detransitive II voice removes the core NP that is lower on the Animacy HeirarchyThe reason that "Passive" and "Antipassive" do not apply is because they are concerned with removing an Agent, in the case of the Passive, or a Patient in the case of the AntipassiveHowever, it is important to remember that the Agent and Patient roles between the two core arguments of a transitive structure are determined by two factors: their positions in the Animacy Heirarchy with respect to one another, and the presence or absence of the Inverse marker ''ta=/tā=/tah=/tāh='' affix on both the main verb and the auxiliary.
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | casipnitá
 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | casipnitá'ita
# ''Nan rompóy yashpa tā'itá' '' "The man hit the dog" (''nan''/man = Agent, ''rompóy''/dog = Patient; Direct Voice - Past Tense)
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tápsipnitá'
# ''Nan rompóy ta-yashpa tahortā'itá' '' "The dog hit the man(''nan''/man = Agent, ''rompóy''/dog = Patient; Inverse Voice - Past Tense)
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tápsipnitá'ita
# ''Rompóy yashpa tākitá' '' "The dog hit" (''tākitá''' = Detransitive I - Past Tense; here, the more animate ''nan''/man was removed, while ''rompóy''/dog which is lower on the Animacy Heirarchy was retained as Agent)
|- 
# ''Nan yashpa hortākitá' '' "The man hit" (''hortākitá''' = Detransitive II - Past Tense; here, the less animate ''rompóy''/dog was removed, and the more animate argument ''nan''/man was retained as Agent)
!  rowspan="11" style="width:64.01pt; " | Medio-Proximal Future
 
|-   
In clause chains, the Detransitive I voice is used to remove the higher animate NP from the argument structure. This allows for the lower animate argument to serve as the pivot for succeeding chains and allows the speaker to apply PRO-dropThe Detransitive II voice, in removing the lower animate argument, ensures that the higher animate NP is retained to serve as the pivot for the succeeding clauses, thus again allowing the speaker to apply PRO-drop without causing ambiguity. If a clause containing a verb in the Detransitive I voice is followed by another clause containing a verb in the Detransitive II voice, or vice versa, the result is similar to languages that use switch-reference to signal which argument is serving the role as Subject.
|  colspan="2" rowspan="3"  style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |
-->
!  colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
 
|- 
===== Finals =====
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
The Irrealis ''-hi'' is suffixed to the copulaIt is obligatory for all Future tensesIt also surfaces in imperatives, yes-no questions which are marked with the ''-yo'' marker cliticizing to the first or second word of a sentence, as in ''Chire-yo tā'itá'-hi?'' "Did he die?", and in WH-questions when the event actually happening has not yet been established as having occurred; for example, ''Enket chire tā'itá'?'', "Where did he die?" implies that the person's death is a fact, and it is only the location that is being inquired, versus ''Enket chire tā'itá'-hi?'' implies that not only is the location unknown, but the person's actual death happening still remains to be established. An interesting structure, called the "double interrogative", is a combination of a WH-word followed by a word bearing the ''-yo'' clitic, which also turns the question into yes-no question: ''Enket chire-yo tā'itá'-hi?'', "Did he die, and if so, where?"   The ''-hi'' suffix is believed to be cognate with the Minhast Irrealis clitic ''=š''.
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
 
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
====Positional and Motion Verbs====
|-
<!--
style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
As an example, the auxiliary ''kohán'' passivizes the clause, resulting in a one-argument clause containing only the logical PatientSince there is only one core argument, the need for inverse marking no longer exists, and in fact the presence of both the passive auxiliary ''kohán'' and the inverse marker ''tā-/tāh-''' in the same sentence is ungrammatical. The same is true for the antipassive auxilliary ''norhe''/''norhâ'', that is the logical Patient is deleted leaving only the agent, and so once again, the presence of the inverse ''tā-/tāh-'' is ungrammatical. When either the Agent or the Patient has been deleted by the passive or antipassive auxiliaries, one thing that must be noted is that the demoted arguments are fully deleted. In other languages that have passives and/or antipassives, demoted core arguments need not be deleted, in fact in many languages the demoted argument may still be retained in the sentence, but this time as an oblique argument. Such is not the case in Nankôre; the former core argument cannot appear in the sentence.
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
The applicative verbs promote an Oblique argument to core status, thereby creating a derived Patient. This results in a transitive sentence, and the inverse marker ''tā-/tāh-'' once again can be used to disambiguate the roles of the core arguments if a lower-animacy argument is an AgentInterestingly, unlike passivation and antipassivation, the demoted core argument, which is always the former Patient, can still appear in the clause as an Oblique argument, marked with the connective ''si-''  There are several of these auxiliaries, listed in the following table:
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
-->
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
The Positional/Motion verbs usually appear at the beginning of the VP.
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
 
|-
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"  
! rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
|+ style="font-weight:bold"| Table of Applicative Auxiliary Verbs
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nitáh
!! Positional/Motion Verb !! Sample Sentence !! Translation !! Comment
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nitahá'ita
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitáh
! Directional
style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitahá'ita
| sanha || Anut America sanha-kor kamuk itá. || Anut supposedly flew to America. ||  ''-kor'' = HEARSAY.EVID, attaches to the first verb of a VP.
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nisiptáh
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nisiptahá'ita
! Benefactive
|-
| nasko|| Nanhoska karen tā-nasko-ro-kor eyna itá. || It is said the (sacred) tree gave (life) for the people.|| Note also the inverse marker ''tā-'' <br/>''-ro-'' = IMPF.
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
style="width:64.01pt; " | nintáh
! Commitative
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nintahá'ita
| yampe || Anut yampe-kor temen itá || Anut walked beside the river with him.|| ''temen'' = to perform an activity by a river
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitahá
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitahá'ita
! Instrumental
| style="width:64.01pt; " | nisipnitáh
| makôr |Ehok yurasna makôr-nahoyra-kor neat itá. || The adolescent struck (it) repeatedly with a club.|| ''-nahoyra-'' = Repetitive <br/> ''neat'', ''ne'at'' = INTENTIVE
style="width:64.01pt; " | nisipnitahá'ita
|-
|-
! Ablative
| colspan="2" rowspan="3"  style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |  
| risa || Shôni Nan Kamun Koryash risa-kor man itá || First Man emerged from (out of) the snow. || ''Shôni Nan'' "First Man" <br/>''Kamun Koryash'' "All Snow" (the birthplace of First Man)
!  colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Patient
|-
|-
! Locative
colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
| neyhi || Yonka asrok tāh-neyhi-kor akun ho itá. || The fly buzzed inside Yonka('s mouth) this morning.|| Note inverse marker ''-tāh''  <br/> ''asrok'' = fly, insect <br/> ''ho'' = earlier this morning.
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
|-
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
! Vialis
|-
| nahke || Pohak tā-nahke-nui shokor-ken ka itá || I saw the river take you through the cave. || ''pohak'' = cave <br/>''shokor'' = to flow<br/>''-ken-'' = CAUS<br/> ''-nui-'' = VIS.EVID
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! Prolative
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| roshkar || Ak piri roshkar hô'itá'. || I crossed the street earlier this morning || ''piri'' = street, road, path
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
! Superessive
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| oros || Ayôhe arupenrô oros-kor yaunke hô'itá.|| The airplane flew over Ayôhe township earlier this morning.|| ''arupenrô'' = airplane <br/> ''yaunke'' = to fly
|-
|-
! rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Agent
! Subessive
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| yorha || Ekurki saska yorha-ro-kor kahoro itá. || The turtle swam underneath the ice.|| ''ekurki'' = turtle <br/>''saska'' = ice<br/>kahoro = ''to swim''  
| style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’nitáh
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tantahá'ita
! Ablative-Superessive
| style="width:64.01pt; " | casnitáh
| isuk || (Example)|| (Example)||
| style="width:64.01pt; " | casnitahá'ita
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tasipnitáh
! Ablative-Subessive
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tasipnitahá'ita
| royna || (Example)|| (Example)||
|-
|-  
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! Riverine
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tānintáh
| temen|| No si-kurasno temen-kor rohnas itá.|| The girls supposedly were playing by the river.||
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tānintahá'ita
|-  
| style="width:64.01pt; " | casnintáh
! Montaigne
| style="width:64.01pt; " | casnintáha'ita
| yayak|| Kahno chôri yayak chire itá || The boy went up the mountain.||
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnitáh
|}
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāsipnintahá'ita
 
|-
These auxiliaries are often confused for adpositions, or relational nouns, but unlike adpositions and locative nouns, these auxiliaries are full-fledged verbsIf inverse marking is required, the prefix ''tā/tāh'' is still prefixed to the auxiliary verbLikewise, aspect and modality markers are attached to the auxiliary, a process that occurs only with verbs in the language, never with nouns. Moreover, these auxiliaries can stand alone as independent verbs, such as ''Kosmakis Anut yampe itá'' "Anut walked with Koshmak" (''yampe '' = to be arm-in-arm; c.f. ''yanup'' "elbow").
! rowspan="11" style="width:64.01pt; " | Immediate Future
 
|-
====Derivation ====
| colspan="2" rowspan="3"  style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |  
 
!  colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
Nankôre has a set of verbalizing prefixes that when attached to nominal roots, often body parts, create verbsThese prefixes are most likely the remnants of Proto-Nahenic noun incorporation, based on comparisons with Nankôre's distant relatives, Minhast and Nahóndasimilar or even identical fossilized affixes have been found in these languages that likewise attach to nominal roots for body parts to derive verbs, e.g. Minhast ''kirim'' (from ''k-erum'', literally "make sound with the mouth) and Nahónda ''teloma'' (from ''t-loma'', note that Minhast /k/ maps to Nahónda /t/); ''kirim'' is the Minhast cognate of Nankôre ''kôre''.
|
 
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
 
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
|+ '''Verbalizing Prefixes'''
|-  
|-  
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
!
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! Prefix
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
! Sample Noun
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! Example
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
! Meaning
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! Nahenic Cognates
|- 
|-
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
! Sound
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| k-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hitáh
| ore "mouth"
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hitahá'ita
| k-ore
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yistáh
| to speak
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yistahá'ita
| Common Minhast ''k-irim'' "to speak" (NB: Nankôre ''ôre'' = "mouth" is cognate with Minhast ''erum'' = "mouth")
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hisipitáh
|-
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hisiptahá'ita
! Striking
|-
| ya-
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| shipa "hand"
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hintáh
| ya-shpa
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hintahá'ita
| to punch
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitáh
| Minhast, Horse Speaker dialect: ''wi-šnu'' "to strike, hit" (Minhast ''išna'' "fist", "knuckles")
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitahá'ita
|-  
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hispintáh
! Movement
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hisipnitahá'ita
| ha-
|-
| nake "foot"
| colspan="2" rowspan="3" style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |  
| ha-nake
!  colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Patient
| to scoot along the floor with one's foot; to kick around
|-
| Nahónda: ''ho-shishpa'' "to give" ''shishp'' = hand; c.f. Minhast ''sespir'' = hand, Nankôre ''shipa'' = hand
! colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
|-
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
! Stability
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
| pa-
|-
| are "eye"
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| p-are
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| to stare
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| Common Minhast ''p-uħta'' "to stand up" (from ''yuħta'' = "sole of the foot", cognate with Nankôre ''yohíhita'' = "flat surface")
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
! Removal
|
| ro-
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Agent
| ampe "body"
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| ro-yampe
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tahitáh
| to remove one's clothes
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tahitahá'ita
| Common Minhast ''ruyyamb'' "to take off clothing" (but note that Minhast lost the original ''*yambet'' (body), replacing it with ''tarti'' (body)
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | castáh
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | castáh
|}
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tahisipitáh
 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tahisiptahá'ita
=== Numbers ===
|-
 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
The Nankôre number system is dozenal, i.e. base-12The numbers thirteen through twenty-three are modified compounds consisting of the unit numbers ''shori'' (1) through ''shiktash'' (11) compounded to ''shin'' (12)The number twenty-four ''hanoshin'' can be analyzed as ''han-o-shin'', where the affix ''-o-'' signifies "multiplied by", so ''hanoshin'' literally means "two times twelve". The multiplicative pattern continues until the number 144, which is called ''nakpa''.  The number 100 (''nenanoshin >> nena ān-o-shin'') is literally "four (and) eight times twelve". The ordinals for the numbers 1-7 are the cardinal numbers suffixed by ''-ak'' or ''-ok''. Numbers 8-11 are the cardinal numbers suffixed with the ending ''-u''. The ordinal for the number 12 is again suffixed with ''-ok'', and the higher numbers with ''-nok''. If the ''-nok'' affix is preceded by a consonant followed by ''-i-'', the ''-i-'' may be dropped, provided that this does not form the impermissible consonant cluster -CCC- sequence.
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhintáh
 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhintahá'ita
{|class="bluetable lightbluebg"  
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | cahsintáh
Number  
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | cahsintahá'ita
Cardinal 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhispintáh
Ordinal    
| style="width:64.01pt; " | tāhisipnitahá'ita
|-  
|-   
one  
!  rowspan="12" style="width:64.01pt; " | Distal Future
| šôri  
|-   
| šoriak   
|  colspan="2" rowspan="3"  style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |
|-
!  colspan="8" style="width:64.01pt; " | Low Animate Patient
two  
|- 
| hani, ôhi  
colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
| hanyak     
colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
|-
colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
!   three  
|-
| tusta, tak  
style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|  tustak   
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-
style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
!   four  
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
| nena, kayoka  
style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| nenak     
style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-  
|-
!   five  
rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
| sišak
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| catak     
style="width:64.01pt; " | pitáh
|-
style="width:64.01pt; " | pitahá'ita
!  six  
style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsitáh
sirišo 
style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsitahá'ita
siiršok
style="width:64.01pt; " | pisiptáh
|-
style="width:64.01pt; " | pisiptahá'ita
!  seven  
|-
kerišo  
style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
kiršok   
style="width:64.01pt; " | pintáh
|-
style="width:64.01pt; " | pintahá'ita
!  eight  
style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsiptáh
| ān  
style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsiptahá'ita
anku       
style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipnitáh
|-
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipnitahá'ita
!  nine  
|- 
karu  
colspan="2" rowspan="3"  style="width:64.01pt; background-color:black " |
| karku     
!  colspan="7" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Patient
|-  
|- 
!   ten  
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
| kaše  
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
kasku     
!  colspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
|-
|- 
! eleven  
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
siktas 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
sistu   
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|-
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
!  twelve  
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
sin  
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
sinok     
|- 
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | High Animate Agent
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | ta’pitáh
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | taptahá'ita
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | capsitáh
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | capsitaháh
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tapsiptáh
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tapsiptahá'ita
|- 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāpintáh
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tāpintahá'ita
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | capsintáh
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | capsintahá'ita
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tápsipnitáh
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | tápsipnitahá'ita
|}
 
‡ Note the irregular Indicative Hodiernal tense form; the Inverse marker does ''not'' lengthen as it does for the rest of the paradigm.
 
=====Intransitive-Active Paradigm =====
 
The paradigm for the ''itá'' auxiliary with intransitive verbs is not as elaborate as its transitive counterpart, nevertheless it still displays a great deal of complexity.  As in the transitive paradigm, an Agent is required as a core argument, but unlike the transitive paradigm, the ''ta-'' inverse marker never appears in the Intransitive-Active paradigm. 
This use of the ''itá'' auxiliary demonstrates an underlying split intransitive morphosyntactic alignment, specifically of the Split-S subtype: only verbs that indicate agency can license the ''itá'' auxiliary, whilst non-volitional verbs are restricted to the ''iná'' auxiliary.
<!--
There is no overt marking on either the main verb or the auxiliary indicating that the NP is an Agent or Patient. Instead, there are intransitive verbs that take only Agents as their core argument, e.g. ''temen'' "to walk downstream" [Agent] vs ''oacir'' "to be carried downriver" [Patient].  The semantic properties of the main verb, rather than morphology, indicate the proper meaning.
-->
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg mw-collapsible"
!  rowspan="1" style="width:64.01pt; " | Tense
!  rowspan="1" style="width:64.01pt; " | Number
!  rowspan="1" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
!  rowspan="1" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
!  rowspan="1" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
|-
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " |  Present
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | itá'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yistá'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | sip'itá' (pis'itá')
|- 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | itán
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitán
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | sipnitán (pisnitán)
|- 
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Hodiernal Past
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hô'itá'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yôsitá'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hôsipnitá'
|- 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hônitán
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yôsintán
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hôsipnintán
|- 
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Past
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | nitá'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitá'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | sipnitá'
|- 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | nintán
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitán
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | sipnintán
|- 
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Medio-Distal Past
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hitá'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yistá'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hispitá'
|- 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hintán
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yisintán
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hisipnitán
|- 
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Distal Past
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pitá'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsitá'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipta
|- 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pintán
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsintán
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipnitán
|-  
|-  
thirteen  
rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Medio-Proximal Future
šorsin  
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
šorsinok  
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | nitáh
|-  
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitáh
!   fourteen  
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | nisiptáh
hansin 
|- 
hanisnok, hanisnok 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-  
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | nintahán
!   fifteen  
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitahán
tusin   
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | nisipnitahán
tusnok
|- 
|-  
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Immediate Future
!   sixteen   
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
ninsin 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hitáh
ninsinok, nisnok 
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yistáh
|-  
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hisiptahán
!   seventeen  
|- 
casin 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
casinok,casnok   
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hintáhan
|-  
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnitahán
eighteen  
style="width:64.01pt; " | hisipnitahán
sirsin  
|-  
sirsinok, sirisnok  
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Distal Future
|-  
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
nineteen  
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pitáh
kirsin    
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsitáh
kirisnok 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | pisiptáh
|-  
|-
!   twenty  
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
ansin   
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pintáhan
asnok   
style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsiptahán
style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipnitahán
|}
 
===== Intransitive-Stative Paradigm=====
Intransitive verb structures involve pairing a semantically stative or non-agentive verb with the auiliary ''iná''', a root cognate with the Minhast intransitive verbal ending ''-an''.
The intransitive paradigm of the auxiliary is listed in the table below: 
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg mw-collapsible"
!  rowspan="1" style="width:64.01pt; " | Tense
!  rowspan="1" style="width:64.01pt; " | Number
!  rowspan="1" style="width:64.01pt; " | Indicative
!  rowspan="1" style="width:64.01pt; " | Inchoative
!  rowspan="1" style="width:64.01pt; " | Causative
|-
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " |  Present
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | iná'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | inán
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | sip'iná' (pis'iná')
|- 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | niná'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yisniná'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | sipniná' (pisniná')
|-
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Hodiernal Past
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
style="width:64.01pt; " | hô'iná'
style="width:64.01pt; " | yôsiná'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hôsipniná'
|-
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hônniná'
style="width:64.01pt; " | yôsinniná'
style="width:64.01pt; " | hôsipnininá'
|-
! rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Past
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | niná'
style="width:64.01pt; " | yisniná'
style="width:64.01pt; " | sipniná'
|-
style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | ninnininá'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yisninniná'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | sipninniná'
|- 
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Medio-Distal Past
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hiná'
style="width:64.01pt; " | yisná'
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hispiná'
|-
style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | hinniná'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yisninniná'
style="width:64.01pt; " | hisipninniná'
|-    
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Distal Past
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | piná'
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsiná'
style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipná'
|-
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|  style="width:64.01pt; " | pinniná'
style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsininniná'
style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipninniná'
|-  
|-  
!   twenty-one  
! rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Medio-Proximal Future
| karusin 
!  style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
karushnok 
style="width:64.01pt; " | nináh
|-
style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnináh
!  twenty-two  
style="width:64.01pt; " | nisipnáh
kasmin 
|-
kasminok 
! style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
|-  
style="width:64.01pt; " | nininnináh
!   twenty-three
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yisninnninahá
siksin  
style="width:64.01pt; " | nisipninnináh
siksinok 
|-  
|-  
!  rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Immediate Future
!   twenty-four  
style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
hanosin  
style="width:64.01pt; " | hináh
hanosnok
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yisnáh
|-  
style="width:64.01pt; " | hisipináh
thirty-six  
|-
tustosin
style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
tustosnok 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | hinnináh
|-  
style="width:64.01pt; " | yisninnináh
one hundred  
style="width:64.01pt; " | hispinnináh
nenanosin
|-
nenanosnok  
rowspan="2" style="width:64.01pt; " | Distal Future
|-  
! style="width:64.01pt; " | SG
!   one hundred forty-four  
style="width:64.01pt; " | pináh
nakpa  
style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsináh
nakpanok 
| style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipnáh
|-
style="width:64.01pt; " | PL
style="width:64.01pt; " | pinninnináh
| style="width:64.01pt; " | yipsipninnináh
style="width:64.01pt; " | pisipninnináh
|}
|}


===Clitics and Particles===
===== Nominalization =====
Nankôre has a rich set of clitics and suffixes that serve as nominalizers.  They are, in order of frequency:
* ''-pe'': Clausal relativizer, cognate with Minhast ''=naft''


====Temporal Deictic Clitics====
{{Gloss
|phrase = Nan issiah pinninnináh'''pe''' pirút taporé ta'itá'.
| IPA =  
| morphemes = nan issiah pinninnináh-pe pirút ta-poré ta-'itá'
| gloss = man come COP.FUT-NMLZ sasquatch INV-vanquish INV-COP
| translation = The (next) man who comes here shall defeat the Sasquatch.
}}
 
* ''-hi''<ref>Not to be confused for Irrealis ''-hi'', which suffixes to the ''itá'/iná' '' auxiliaries.</ref><ref>Cognate with Minhast gerundial ''-x-''</ref>: attaches directly to a verb root to derive participles
* ''-hici'': Derives instruments from verb roots, a compound of ''-hi-'' and a submorpheme ''-ci'', possibly derived from ''kaci'' "path"
* ''-rē''<ref>Cognate with Minhast ''redad'' "man/male/person</ref> : Derives occupation
* ''-ssiak'' <ref> Derived from ''siriak'', "appointed time". Cognate with Minhast ''saħrap'' "today"</ref> Derives temporal adverbs from verbs, e.g. ''petissiak'' "time of/for standing", as well as creating adverbial clauses when attached to ''itá '' or ''iná '', e.g.
 
{{Gloss
|phrase = Ak saraka nitássiak, sissioyenki kai pohráp niná'
| IPA =  
| morphemes = ak saraka n-itá-ssiak, si=sioyenki kai pohráp n-iná'
| gloss = 1S see PST-AUX.TRN CONN=heart down fall PST-INTR
| translation = When I saw him, I became crestfallen.
}}
 
The adverbial clause and its matrix may be inverted, e.g.:
 
{{Gloss
|phrase = Sioyenki kai pohráp ninássiak', siak saraka nitá.
| IPA =
| morphemes = sioyenki kai pohráp n-iná'-ssiak' si=ak saraka n-itá.
| gloss = heart down fall PST-INTR CONN=1S see PST-AUX.TRN CONN=
| translation = I became crestfallen, having seen him.
}}
 
The Connective ''si='' obligatorily surfaces to link the adverbial clause with its matrix, regardless of the position of the two clauses.
 
====Derivation ====
 
Nankôre has a set of verbalizing prefixes that when attached to nominal roots, often body parts, create verbs.  These prefixes are most likely the remnants of Proto-Nahenic noun incorporation, based on comparisons with Nankôre's distant relatives, Minhast and Nahónda;  similar or even identical fossilized affixes have been found in these languages that likewise attach to nominal roots for body parts to derive verbs.<ref> C.f. Minhast ''kirim'' (from ''k-erum'', literally "make sound with the mouth) and Nahónda ''teloma'' (from ''t-loma'').  Both are cognates of Nankôre's ''kôre'' "speech". </ref>  <!--Note that Minhast /k/ maps to Nahónda /t/). ''kirim'' is the Minhast cognate of Nankôre ''kôre''. -->


The Temporal Deitic clitics are usually attached to the first word of a sentence, e.g. ''Anut=antak America san-kamuk tā'itá' '', "A year ago, Anut flew to America".  In multiclausal sentences, the clause in which they appear have scope over the elements preceding it. At least one, ''=yorai'', cannot appear after the initial clause.  These clitics may also be attached to the main verb, and in the process replace the auxiliary tense affixes of the Direct Voice, as in ''Anut America san-kamuk=inkát itá' '' "Several years ago Anut flew to America" (note that the Temporal Deictic clitic ''=inkát'' has replaced the past tense affix ''tā-'' in ''tā'itá' ''). Compare this with the Direct Voice Distal Past form ''suphitá' '', in ''Anut America san-kamuk suphitá' '', "Anut went to America a while back".  Also note that the other verb voices do not lose their tense markers, as in ''Otok erashi ta-karok=inkat ta'itá''' (boy crab INV-eat=several.years.ago INV.PST-COP) "The crab ate the boy."


{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg"
|+ '''Verbalizing Prefixes'''
|-
!
! Prefix
! Sample Noun
! Example
! Meaning
! Nahenic Cognates
|-
|-
! Clitic
! Sound
! Meaning
| k-
! Header text
| ore "mouth"
|-
| k-ore
|  =yorai
| to speak
| Once upon a time
| Common Minhast ''k-irim'' "to speak" (NB: Nankôre ''ôre'' = "mouth" is cognate with Minhast ''erum'' = "mouth")
| Example
|-
|-
! Striking
| =(i)nkát || Several years ago  || Example
| ya-
| sipa "hand"
| ya-spa
| to punch
| Minhast, Horse Speaker dialect: ''wi-šnu'' "to strike, hit" (Minhast ''išna'' "fist", "knuckles")
|-
! Movement
| ho-<br/>ha-
| nake "foot"
| ha-nake
| to scoot along the floor with one's foot; to kick around
| Nahónda: ''ho-shishpa'' "to give" ''shishp'' = hand; c.f. Minhast ''sespir'' = hand, Nankôre ''sipa'' = hand
|-
|-
| =sheyhori || A few years ago || Example
! Stability
|-
| pa-
| =(a)ntak ||A year ago ||  Example
| are "eye"
|-
| p-are
|  =(e)kak || Several days ago, last week || Example
| to stare
|-
| Common Minhast ''p-uħta'' "to stand up" (from ''yuħta'' = "sole of the foot", cognate with Nankôre ''yohíhita'' = "flat surface")
| =nate || The day before yesterday ||  Example
|-
| =kira || Yesterday || Example
|-
| =ho || Earlier this morning || Example
|-
| =tora || Just awhile ago || Example
|-
| =(e)yót || Now || Example
|-
| =(e)nash || Soon || Example
|-
| =k(e)sor || Tomorrow || Example
|-
| =n(i)kos || In the next few days || Example
|-
| =n(a)koy || In the indeterminate future || Example
|-
|-
! Removal
| ro-
| ampe "body"
| ro-yampe
| to remove one's clothes
| Common Minhast ''ruyyamb'' "to take off clothing" (but note that Minhast lost the original ''*yambet'' (body), replacing it with ''tarti'' (body)
|-
|}
|}


=== Numbers ===


<!--
The Nankôre number system is dozenal, i.e. base-12.  The numbers thirteen through twenty-three are modified compounds consisting of the unit numbers ''siori'' (1) through ''siktas'' (11) compounded to ''sin'' (12).  The number twenty-four ''hanosin'' can be analyzed as ''han-o-sin'', where the affix ''-o-'' signifies "multiplied by", so ''hanosin'' literally means "two times twelve".  The multiplicative pattern continues until the number 144, which is called ''nakpa''.  The number 100 (''nenanosin >> nena ān-o-sin'') is literally "four (and) eight times twelve".  The ordinals for the numbers 1-7 are the cardinal numbers suffixed by ''-ak'' or ''-ok''. Numbers 8-11 are the cardinal numbers suffixed with the ending ''-u''. The ordinal for the number 12 is again suffixed with ''-ok'', and the higher numbers with ''-nok''If the ''-nok'' affix is preceded by a consonant followed by ''-i-'', the ''-i-'' may be dropped, provided that this does not form the impermissible consonant cluster -CCC- sequence.
tak orâshim kot "3 Orashim trees" = 3 hundredAn orashim tree is apporximately the length of 100 hand lengths (from heel of wrist to the tip of the longest, i.e. 3rd, finger)An orashim is also 100.
-->


==Syntax==
{|class="bluetable lightbluebg"
 
!  Number  
====Word Order====
!  Cardinal 
 
!  Ordinal    
Nankôre is classified as a SOV language. The auxiliary verbs, such as the copula ''itá'', obligatorily appear in clause-final position. Although the core argument NPs do not take any overt case marking, the animacy hierarchy and inverse system usually provide enough information to identify the Agent from the Patient, so OSV orders are also found in speech and text. Although auxiliary verbs are obligatorily clause-final, the main verb may appear at the beginning of the clause for emphasis, as in ''Kakno, rihat makshe no itá'' >> seize.3, falcon.AGT mouse.PT SEM COP, lit. "It seizes it, the falcon the mouse in.one.act it.does", i.e. "The falcon seizes the mouse in one fell swoop." Note that the fronted verb is followed by an audible pause.
|-
 
! one  
As in many SOV languages, modifiers precede their heads, e.g. ''Shirkosh shi=rohpa,'' "This fine foreign cloth" (lit. "Foreigner fine.cloth").
|  siôri  
 
|  sioriak   
====The Noun Phrase====
|-  
 
!  two  
====The Verb Phrase====
| hani, ôhi  
 
|  hanyak     
The verb phrase may contain up to three independent verb stems or roots. All verb phrases must end with the appropriate form of the auxiliary ''itá''. ''Itá'' was originally a copula but later on accreted voice, tense, and the secondary ''ta-'' Inverse marker. The main verb precedes the auxiliary and may be separated from it by a de-focused noun or by one or more adjuncts. De-focused nouns appear immediately after the main verb and adjuncts cannot be inserted between the two (see "Quasi-Incorporation" below). Positional or motion verbs appear before the main verb. The primary ''ta-'' Inverse marker cliticizes to the first verb of the verb phrase.
|-
 
!  three  
=====Quasi-Incorporation=====
|  tusta, tak  
 
|  tustak   
Nankôre canonical word order is SOV<sub>1</sub>V<sub>2</sub>, but under certain circumstances, the O-argument may occur between V<sub>1</sub> and V<sub>2</sub>, i.e. SV<sub>1</sub>OV<sub>2</sub>.  The O-argument becomes backgrounded, thereby assuming a peripheral role; as a result, the clause is effectively detransitivized. The Inverse marker ''ta-'' does not occur in intransitive clauses; it must be omitted for the clause to remain grammatical.  This operation is called ''quasi-incorporation'', and occurs cross-linguistically in unrelated languages, e.g. Dutch and Hungarian.  A feature of Nankôre quasi-incorporation is that adjuncts, which typically can occur in most positions of a clause, including clause-finally, cannot occur between V<sub>1</sub> and the relocated O-argument.
|-
 
!  four  
Quasi-incorporation may eventually evolve into full noun incorporation, as in the case of Nankôre's distant relatives, Minhast and NahóndaBoth exhibit full noun incorporation.  Like Nankôre, both languages add the incorporated noun after the verb root, e.g. Minhast ''Ušnirupmakekaru'' << ''ušn-ruppumak-ek-ar-u'' (hit-face-3S.ABS+1S.ERG-PST.PFCT-TRANS) "I hit him in the face, I face-hit him",  Nahónda ''klomenatsoyetolayo''<< ''kloma-natsoye-Ø-t-ola-yo'' (speak-wisdom-3S.PT-1S.AGT-PST-AGT) "I gave him counsel, I wisdom-speak to him.  Most languages that exhibit noun incorporation attach the incorporated noun ''before'' the verb root; placement of the incorporated noun after the root is rare among incorporating languages.  The process by which Nankôre performs quasi-incorporation, namely by placing the O-argument ''between'' the primary verb and auxiliary verbs, may explain why Minhast and Nahónda place their incorporated nouns after the verb.  Proto-Nahenic may have also placed a quasi-incorporated noun after the primary verb but before the auxiliary verb, an order which the descendant languages preserved.
|  nena, kayoka  
 
| nenak     
====Possession====
|-  
 
!  five  
Possession is indicated by joining the possessor to the possessum with the connective ''si=/shi='', followed either by the verb ''ras/rash'' to indicate inalienable possession e.g. ''Koykare shi=naho rash'', "The boy's mother", or ''ocité'' for alienable possession, e.g. ''Koykare shi=mashka  ocité''.  If the possessor is lower in the animacy heirarcy, the inverse marker ''ta'/tah'' is prefixed to the verb, as in the improbable ''Mashka shi=koykare tah-ocité'', "The anthill's boy".
|  sisiak
 
|  catak     
====Comparatives and Superlatives====
|-
 
!  six  
Degrees of comparison are formed by placing the auxiliary verb ''soyka'' (to be big, i.e. to be more) or ''hatka'' (to be small, i.e. to be less) before a stative verb.  Since two arguments are involved, namely the comparer NP and the compared NP, the phrase is structurally equivalent to a transitive clause, requiring an Agent and a Patient, and follows the rules governing the nominal animacy hierarchy.  In the sentence ''Joe Sara soyka inupe tā itá'' (Joe is stronger than Sarah; lit. "Joe bigs strongs Sara"), ''soyka'' precedes the stative verb ''inupe'' (to be strong)Joe, being male, is higher in the animacy scale and so is assigned Agent status. If Sara were stronger, the Inverse affix ''tā-/tāh-'' surfaces, as in ''Joe Sara '''tā'''-soyka inupe  tā itá'' (Sara is stronger than Joe)Other examples:
|  sirisio 
 
|  siirsiok
1) ''Joe Sara soyka soyka tā itá'' (Joe is taller than Sara; lit. "Joe bigs bigs Sara").
|-
 
!  seven  
2) ''Joe Sara tā-hatka nahamosh tā itá'' (Sara is less tan than Joe).
|  kerisio  
 
|  kirsiok   
3) ''Joe Sara hatka hatka tā itá'' (Joe is smaller than Sara; lit. "Joe small smalls Sara").
|-
 
!  eight  
4) ''Natos Suhe soyka makit itá'' (Natosh [masc.] runs faster than Suhe [fem.]; lit. "Natosh big runs Suhe").
|  ān  
 
|  anku       
5) ''Orôyo Paul tā-soyka soyka itá'' (The volcano is bigger than Paul; note that ''orôyo'' is less animate because it is a sessile object, hence the use of the inverse marker ''tā-'').  
|-
 
!  nine  
 
|  karu  
To express the superlative, the clause must be antipassivized with the auxiliary verb ''norhe/norhâ'' e.g. ''Orôyo norhe soyka soyka itá'' (This is the largest mountain), or ''Sikôya orôyo norhe soyka soyka itá'' (This is the largest mountain in Kôya Island).
|  karku     
 
|-
 
!  ten  
<!-- Example: Word order, qualifiers, determinatives, branching, etc. -->
|  kase /'kaʃɛ/
 
|  kasku     
===Constituent order===
|-
===Dependent clauses===
!  eleven  
<!-- etc. etc. -->
|  siktas 
 
|  sistu   
==Example texts==
|-
==Other resources==
!  twelve  
<!-- Example: Word order, qualifiers, determinatives, branching, etc. -->
|  sin  
 
|  sinok     
 
|-
 
!  thirteen  
==(Phrases and terms from FB - To be incorporated later) ==
|  siorsin 
 
|  siorsinok 
 
|-
1) "tak orâshim kot": 3 Orashim trees hands" (i.e. the number 300). The length of the trunk of an orâshim tree is the length of approximately 100 hand-lenghths, from heel of the hand to tip of the middle finger.
!  fourteen  
 
|  hansin 
2) "koytan karásh", meaning "lava lizard", a scarlet colored giant salamander with venomous glands scattered across its skin. It lives in certain mountain rivers in Kôya Island  
|  hanisnok, hanisnok 
|-
!  fifteen  
|  tusin   
|  tusnok
|-
!  sixteen   
|  ninsin 
|  ninsinok, nisnok 
|-
!  seventeen  
|  casin 
|  casinok,casnok   
|-
! eighteen  
|  sirsin 
|  sirsinok, sirisnok 
|-
!  nineteen  
|  kirsin 
|  kirisnok 
|-
!  twenty  
|  ansin   
|  asnok   
|-
!  twenty-one  
|  karusin 
|  karushnok 
|-
!  twenty-two  
| kasmin 
|  kasminok 
|-
!  twenty-three
|  siksin 
|  siksinok 
|-
!  twenty-four 
|  hanosin  
|  hanosnok
|-  
! thirty-six 
|  tustosin
|  tustosnok 
|-  
!  one hundred  
|  nenanosin
|  nenanosnok  
|-  
!  one hundred forty-four 
|  nakpa 
|  nakpanok 
|}
 
===Clitics and Particles===
 
==== Connective ''si='' ====
Throughout all the Nahenic languages, connectives are used to bind dependents to their heads.  Minhast has ''min'', Neina the cognate ''mɯn'', and Nahónda ''na'', to join two or more noun phrases into one cohesive unit. Minhast has one additional connective, ''wa='', which joins non-verbal adjuncts, which may be a single word or an entire NP and/or adverbial phrase.
 
Nankóre's connective ''si='' has no known cognates, and unlike its sister languages, ''si='' is not restricted to joining NPs or complex, non-verbal phrases.  While the dependency relation in a ''si='' construction may be discernible, often the directionality is ambiguous and thus context must be used to disambiguate, or other morphosyntactic devices, such as the co-occurrence of the inverse marker ''ta' '' in possessive phrases.
 
<!--
Interestingly, some NP phrases where one would expect a connective to appear are absentA notable example is ''Hôkun Pe' '', literally "long house", rather than the expected ''hokun si-Pe' '', which is uncommon. The expected form is ''Pe' hokun iná' '', lit. "The house (that) is long".
-->
 
====Other Particles====
{|class="bluetable lightbluebg"
|-
! style="width: 100px;" | Particle
! Meaning
! Cognates
|-
! ea<br/>ira
| yes
| Upper Minhast ''ēlā''
|-
! ecá'<br/> icá'
| no
| Common Minhast ''hatā'
|-
! na
| and <br/> so, therefore
| Nahónda ''máma'' "also", Common Minhast ''=mā'' "and then"
|-
! piya
| but
| Neina ''(hi)yo''
|-
! kusur
| then
| Neina ''kisero'' "afterwards"
|-
! ritá'a
| however
| Neina ''riha'' "to turn", Nahónda ''letsaya'' "to twist", Upper Minhast ''ruttakšuran'' "to seduce" (from Classical Minhast ''ruttay-gurra-ša'ra-ana'' "to swing one's hips")
|}
 
<!--
tak orâshim kot "3 Orashim trees" = 3 hundred.  An orashim tree is apporximately the length of 100 hand lengths (from heel of wrist to the tip of the longest, i.e. 3rd, finger). An orashim is also 100.
-->
 
==Syntax==
<!-- Example: Word order, qualifiers, determinatives, branching, etc. -->
====Word Order====
 
Nankôre's canonical word order SOV, more specifically, SOV<sub>1</sub>V<sub>2</sub>, where V<sub>2</sub> represents the auxiliary ''itá''.  Although the core argument NPs do not take any overt case marking, the animacy hierarchy and direct-inverse system of marking usually provide enough information to identify the Agent from the Patient.  Thus, OSV<sub>1</sub>V<sub>2</sub>, SVO<sub>1</sub> V<sub>2</sub>, and other orders are found, both in speech and text.  When pronouns appear as core arguments, they usually appear before the main verb, but they may cliticize to the end of the main verb:
 
{{Gloss
|phrase = Minhaste kôryak itá'
|IPA = /'minɦaʃte 'ko:rjak ɪt'aʔ/
| morphemes = minhast kôre=ak ∅-∅-itá-ʔ
| gloss = Minhast.language  speak=1S DIR-HS-COP-LS
| translation = I speak the Minhast language.
}}
   
Nevertheless, for all word order variants, the only restriction is the auxiliary is restricted to clause-final position.
 
Fronting a constituent is used for emphasis, as in the following sample, where the O argument has been fronted before the S argument.  Fronted constituents are typically followed by an audible pause, as in the following example:
 
{{Gloss
|phrase = Makse, rihat kakno itá
|IPA = /'makʃɛ̯ ɾi'hat 'kakno ɪt'aʔ/
| morphemes = makse rihat kak=no ∅-∅-itá
| gloss = mouse.LA falcon.HA seize=SEM DIR-HS-COP-LS
| translation = It was the mouse the falcon seized in one fell swoop.
}}
 
As in many SOV languages, modifiers precede their heads:
 
{{Gloss
|phrase = Sirkos sirohpa
|IPA = /'ʃirkos ʃiroɦpa/
| morphemes = sirkos si=rohpa
| gloss = foreigner fine.cloth
| translation = This fine foreign cloth
}}
 
====The Noun Phrase====
 
=====Possession=====
Possession is indicated by joining the possessor to the possessum with the connective ''si='', followed either by the verb ''ras'' to indicate inalienable possession e.g. ''Koykare si=naho ras'', "The boy's mother", or ''ocité'' for alienable possession, e.g. ''Koykare si=maska  ocité''.  If the possessor is lower in the animacy heirarcy, the inverse marker ''ta'/tah'' is prefixed to the verb, as in the improbable ''Maska si=koykare tah-ocité'', "The anthill's boy".
 
====The Verb Phrase====
 
The verb phrase is made up of the [[Nankôre#The_Verb_Complex|verb complex]], and its predicate.  The verb complex must end with the appropriate form of the auxiliary ''itá''. ''Itá'' was originally a auxiliary but later on accreted voice, tense, and the pleonastic ''ta-'' Inverse marker.  The main verb precedes the auxiliary and may be separated from it by a de-focused noun or by one or more adjuncts.  De-focused nouns appear immediately after the main verb, and adjuncts cannot be inserted between the two (see "Quasi-Incorporation" below).  Coverbs  appear before the main verb.  The primary ''ta-'' Inverse marker cliticizes to the first verb of the verb complex, which may be either a coverb, or the main verb.
 
=====Quasi-Noun Incorporation=====
 
Nankôre canonical word order is SOV<sub>1</sub>V<sub>2</sub>, but under certain circumstances, the O-argument may occur between V<sub>1</sub> and V<sub>2</sub>, i.e. SV<sub>1</sub>OV<sub>2</sub>.  The O-argument becomes backgrounded, thereby assuming a peripheral role; as a result, the clause is effectively detransitivized.  As a result, the Inverse marker ''ta-'' cannot occur in these derived intransitive clauses; it must be omitted for the clause to remain grammatical.  This operation is called ''quasi-noun incorporation'', and occurs cross-linguistically in unrelated languages, e.g. Dutch and Hungarian.  A feature of Nankôre psuedo-incorporation is that adjuncts, which typically can occur in most positions of a clause, including clause-finally, cannot occur between V<sub>1</sub> and the relocated O-argument. 
 
Quasi-incorporation may eventually evolve into full noun incorporation, as in the case of Nankôre's distant relatives, Minhast and Nahónda.  Both exhibit full noun incorporation.  Like Minhast and Nahónda, Nankôre  adds the incorporated noun after the verb root, e.g. Minhast ''Ušnirumpakekaru'' << ''ušn-ruppumak-ek-ar-u'' (hit-face-3S.ABS+1S.ERG-PST.PFCT-TRANS) "I hit him in the face, I face-hit him",  Nahónda ''klomenatsoyetolayo''<< ''kloma-natsoye-Ø-t-ola-yo'' (speak-wisdom-3S.PT-1S.AGT-PST-AGT) "I gave him counsel, I wisdom-speak to him.  Most languages that exhibit noun incorporation attach the incorporated noun ''before'' the verb root; placement of the incorporated noun after the root is rare among incorporating languages.  The process by which Nankôre performs pseudo-incorporation, namely by placing the O-argument ''between'' the primary verb and auxiliary verbs, may explain why Minhast and Nahónda place their incorporated nouns after the verb.  Proto-Nahenic may have also placed a quasi-incorporated noun after the primary verb but before the auxiliary verb, an order which the descendant languages preserved.
Quasi-Noun Incorporation serves discourse functions similar to full noun incorporation found in its Minhast and Nahónda relatives, backgrounding the incorporated NP, decreasing valency, and providing additional specificity to the semantics of the main verb.  The following example of compares and contrasts the default SOV<sub>1</sub>V<sub>2</sub> word order, with that of a SV<sub>1</sub>OV<sub>2</sub>:
 
*Default SOV<sub>1</sub>V<sub>2</sub> Order
 
{{Gloss
|phrase = Makse rihat tayôreno ta'itá
|IPA = /'makʃɛ̯ ɾi'hat ta'yo:reno taʔɪt'aʔ/
| morphemes = makse rihat ta=yôre=no  ta-itá-ʔ
| gloss = mouse.LA falcon.HA  INV=bite=SEM INV-HS.COP-LS
| translation = The mouse bit the falcon.
}}
 
*Quasi-Noun Incorporation,  SV<sub>1</sub>OV<sub>2</sub> Order
 
{{Gloss
|phrase = Makse yôreno rihat 'itá'
|IPA = /'makʃɛ̯ 'yo:reno ɾi'hat taʔɪt'aʔ/
| morphemes = makse yôre=no rihat  ∅-itá-ʔ
| gloss = mouse.LA bite=SEM falcon DIR-COP-LS
| translation = The mouse falcon-bit.
}}
 
Quasi-noun incorporation, like the full noun incorporation found in Nankôre's relatives, Minhast and Nahónda, lowers valency, transforming transitive clauses to intransitive clauses.  '''The use of the Inverse marker is no longer needed''', as the incorporated noun has been reduced to an adjunct.  As such, the incorporated noun no longer occupies a core position. Decreasing the clause's valency reduces the saliency of the incorporated noun.
 
====Comparatives and Superlatives====
 
Degrees of comparison are formed by placing the coverb ''soyka'' (to be big, i.e. to be more) or ''hatka'' (to be small, i.e. to be less) before a stative verb.  Since two arguments are involved, namely the comparer NP and the compared NP, the phrase is structurally equivalent to a transitive clause, requiring an Agent and a Patient, and follows the rules governing the nominal animacy hierarchy.  In the sentence ''Joe Sara soyka inupe itá'' (Joe is stronger than Sarah; lit. "Joe bigs strongs Sara"), ''soyka'' precedes the stative verb ''inupe'' (to be strong).  Joe, being male, is higher in the animacy scale and so is assigned Agent status.  If Sara were stronger, the Inverse affix ''ta-/tā-/tāh-'' surfaces, as in ''Joe Sara '''ta'''-soyka inupe  ta'itá'' (Sara is stronger than Joe).  Other examples:
 
1) ''Joe Sara soyka soyka ta'itá'' (Joe is taller than Sara; lit. "Joe big bigs Sara").
 
2) ''Joe Sara tā-hatka nahamos ta'itá'' (Sara is less tan than Joe).
 
3) ''Joe Sara hatka hatka ta'itá'' (Joe is smaller than Sara; lit. "Joe small smalls Sara").
 
4) ''Natos Suhe soyka makit itá'' (Natosh [masc.] runs faster than Suhe [fem.]; lit. "Natosh big runs Suhe").
 
5) ''Orôyo Paul ta-soyka soyka itá'' (The volcano is bigger than Paul; note that ''orôyo'' is less animate because it is a sessile object, hence the use of the inverse marker ''ta-'').
 
To express the superlative, the auxiliary verb ''norhe/norhâ'' appears before ''soyka'' and ''hatka'', e.g. ''Orôyo norhe soyka soyka itá'' (This is the largest mountain), or ''Sikôya orôyo norhe soyka soyka itá'' (This is the largest mountain in Kôya Island).
 
==Example texts==
=== The Tower of Babel ===
# Now the whole earth had one language and the same words. 
# And as people migrated from the east, they found a plain in the land of Shinar and settled there.
# And they said to one another, "Come, let us make bricks, and burn them thoroughly." And they had brick for stone, and bitumen for mortar.
# Then they said, "Come, let us build ourselves a city and a tower with its top in the heavens, and let us make a name for ourselves, lest we be dispersed over the face of the whole earth."
# And the Lord came down to see the city and the tower, which the children of man had built.
# And the Lord said, "Behold, they are one people, and they have all one language, and this is only the beginning of what they will do. And nothing that they propose to do will now be impossible for them.
# Come, let us go down and there confuse their language, so that they may not understand one another's speech."
# So the Lord dispersed them from there over the face of all the earth, and they left off building the city.
# Therefore its name was called Babel, because there the Lord confused the language of all the earth. And from there the Lord dispersed them over the face of all the earth.
<br/>
# Saska nammanuyyi nan siôri si-kôre maskoy pintá', kor-kôre nihe pinninnináh.<br/>Now in.those.days mankind one CONN-speech have AUX.TRN+DIST.PST+HP.LS, word-word resemble AUX.INTR+DIST.PST.PL
# Iape nanni hóttari pinninnináh, coy siori pintá', Sinar rima pintá', itáp pinninnináh. <br/> So horde approach.setting.sun AUX.INTR+DIST.PST.PL, forest.clearing see AUX.TRN+DIST.PST+HP.LS, Sinar call AUX.TRN+DIST.PST+HP.LS, sit.down AUX.INTR+DIST.PST.PL
 
== Table of Abbreviations ==
 
{| class="bluetable lightbluebg sortable mw-collapsible"
!Abbreviation
!Meaning
|-
! style="text-align:left" | C
| Single Consonant
|-
! style="text-align:left" | V
| Single Vowel
|-
! style="text-align:left" | VV
| Long/Geminate Vowel
|-
! style="text-align:left" | PN
| Proper Noun
|-
! style="text-align:left" | NP
| Noun Phrase
|-
! style="text-align:left" | VP
| Verb Phrase
|-
! style="text-align:left" | OBL
| Oblique argument
|-
! style="text-align:left" | DEM
| Demonym
|-
! style="text-align:left" | AGT
| Agent
|-
! style="text-align:left" | PT
| Patient
|-
! style="text-align:left" | DIR
| Direct Voice
|-
! style="text-align:left" | INV
| Inverse Voice
|-
! style="text-align:left" | CONN
| Connective
|-
! style="text-align:left" | HA
| High Animate
|-
! style="text-align:left" | HS
| High Animate Singular
|-
! style="text-align:left" | HP
| High Animate Plural
|-
! style="text-align:left" | LA
| Low Animate
|-
! style="text-align:left" | LS
| Low Animate Singular
|-
! style="text-align:left" | LP
| Low Animate Plural
|-
! style="text-align:left" | NEG
| Negator
|-
! style="text-align:left" | CAUS
| Causative
|-
! style="text-align:left" | INCH
| Inchoative
|-
! style="text-align:left" | SEM
| Semelfective
|-
! style="text-align:left" | PNI
| Pseudo-Noun Incorporation
|-
! style="text-align:left" | PIN
| Pseudo-Incorporated Noun
|-
! style="text-align:left" | SG
| Singular
|-
! style="text-align:left" | PL
| Plural
|-
! style="text-align:left" | REM.PST
| Remote Past Tense
|-
! style="text-align:left" | HOD
| Hodiurnal Past Tense
|-
! style="text-align:left" | PST
| Past Tense
|-
! style="text-align:left" | PRS
| Present Tense
|-
! style="text-align:left" | IMM.FUT
| Immediate Future Tense
|-
! style="text-align:left" | FUT
| Future Tense
|-
! style="text-align:left" | IMPF
| Imperfect Aspect
|-
! style="text-align:left" | PRF
| Perfect Aspect
|-
! style="text-align:left" | IRR
| Irrealis
|}
 
==Other resources==
<!-- Example: Word order, qualifiers, determinatives, branching, etc. -->
 
 
 
 
<!--
==(Phrases and terms from FB - To be incorporated later) ==
 
 
1) "tak orâshim kot": 3 Orashim trees hands" (i.e. the number 300). The length of the trunk of an orâshim tree is the length of approximately 100 hand-lenghths, from heel of the hand to tip of the middle finger.
 
2) "koytan karásh", meaning "lava lizard", a scarlet colored giant salamander with venomous glands scattered across its skin. It lives in certain mountain rivers in Kôya Island  


koytan = lava
koytan = lava
Line 1,832: Line 2,462:


9)
9)
 
-->




Line 1,878: Line 2,508:


[[Category:Languages]]
[[Category:Languages]]
[[Category:Conlangs]]
[[Category:Artlangs]]
[[Category:Artlangs]]
[[Category:A priori]]
[[Category:A priori]]

Latest revision as of 20:22, 23 March 2024


Nankôre
Nankôre
Pronunciation[nan.'ko:re]
Created byAnyar
SettingPacific Northwest
Native toNanhoska Kôya /nan'hoʃka 'ko:ja/
EthnicityNanhoska people
Native speakers2,324 (2014)
Nahenic
  • Shakhtabaric
    • Nankoric
      • Nankôre
Official status
Official language in
Koya Island



Introduction

Nankôre, from the words nan ("man, human") and kôre ("speech"), is spoken by the Nanhoshka people of Kôya Island. There are two main dialects, the Konishmak, located in the Northeastern Coastal Mountains, and the Sapshira, encompassing the western and southern parts of the country. The dialects differ chiefly in pronunciation and differences in certain lexical items, but are otherwise mutually intelligible.

For a little over a century the origins of the language and its people remain clouded in mystery. Physically, the Nanhoshka people look distinct from the Northwest Pacific Native Americans just 862 kilometers from the eastern shore, but their physical features bear a strikingly close resemblance to the Minhast people of the Minhastim Karak, who live much further away on the other side of the Pacific Ocean close to Japan and Moshir Ainu. Because of these physical resemblances, both anthropologists and linguists started exploring a possible relationship between the two peoples. Recent genetic research uncovered that both the Nanhoshka and Minhast belong to the haplogroup C-M212, indicating a common ancestry. Linguists had a more difficult task in establishing a relationship. Finally Benson et. al. compiled an extensive Swadesh list and internally consistent sound correspondences between the two languages. Most importantly, a set of affixes that were quite conservative were revealed, notably the Causative affix, and a set of fossilized affixes which are prefixed to body parts to derive verbs. In addition, a third language, Nahónda, also previously classified as a language isolate, was discovered to contain common vocabulary and affixes which it shared with both Nankôre and Minhast. Therefore, both Nankôre and Minhast, along with Nahónda, are now recognized as belonging to the same language family, which has been named the Nahenic language family.

Typology and Grammatical Overview

Like Minhast and Nahónda, Nankôre is an SOV language. However, unlike its relatives, Nankôre is not a polysynthetic language but is relatively sparse in terms of affixes for determining syntactic relations. Nouns do not receive case or number marking, nor are they marked for gender. Word order within the verb complex is rather strict, but core, patient, and obliques may occur in various orders. The verb complex consists minimally of a main verb, which possesses suffixes for marking evidentiality, and the auxiliary, which contain a rich set of affixes to mark the core arguments for various syntactic features: number, tense, voice, agency, etc.

Nankôre displays a mixed morphosyntactic alignment. Particularly noticeable is the Hierarchical alignment in its transitive verbs, which employ Direct-Inverse marking to indicate core argument roles. The nominal hierarchy is complex; at least ten noun classes based on animacy can be identified, and within those classes sub-hierarchies are observed based on semantic features, although discourse processes (e.g. the introduction of new arguments that are roughly equal in animacy with previously established arguments, or a previous argument that serves as the topic over extended discourse) may affect how direct-inverse marking is expressed. Thus hierarchical relations are primarily dependent on the semantics of the core argument and the verb class, although other factors are at play; native speakers intuit the hierarchy based on a complex interaction between the both the noun and verb classes, discourse patterns, and context.

The language also displays an Active-Stative alignment of the Split-S subtype in its intransitive verbs. As in its relative Nahónda, Nankôre intransitive verbs are split into two categories, one that requires an Agent as the sole core argument, and another that takes only Patients. Two different auxiliary paradigms are used for intransitive verb roots, the itá paradigm for agentive verbs, and the iná paradigm for stative verbs.

Phonology

Orthography

The Nankôre orthography is based on the Allen-Mills system, derived from the Latin alphabet. However, a second system, the American Media system, has started displacing the Allen-Mills system. Although based on the Allen-Mills system, the American Media system uses the the digraphs <ch> and <sh> to make transcription of the language more accessible to American readers.

Allen-Mills American Media IPA
' (n/a) ʔ
a,ā, â a,ā,â a, a:
e e e
i i ɪ, i
o, ô o, ô o, o:
u u u
c ch
s sh s, s̺, ʃ
ś, š[1] sh ʃ
h h ɦ
k k k
r r r
m m m
n n n
p p p
t t t
y y j


Note that the grapeme <s> in the Allen-Mills system is pronounced /ʃ/ or /s̺/ when followed by [i] (e.g. sinkokah /ʃin'kokah/ "orca"), or in coda position (Nanhoska /nan'hoʃka/ "the True People"). A few decades after the Allen-Mills transcription system was developed, a sound shift appeared in the northwestern Hoyampe dialect, whereby /s/ shifted to the voiceless apico-alveolar fricative /s̺/ when followed by /i/ or in coda position. A similar sound shift occurred at around the same time in the northeastern Konishmak dialect (possibly the result of the Hoyampe sound shift), but it was the voiceless palato-alveolar fricative /ʃ/ that appeared. It was this sound shift that spread out through most of the country, although there are some remote regions of the country that have not been affected by either sound shift. As a result in foreign transcriptions <sh> is often used in words where the /s/ >> /ʃ/ has already occurred, e.g. akôsh vs. the original akôs transcription. The grapheme <ô> is used to indicate vowel length for /o:/, and <ā> for /ɑ:/.

Although the American Media orthography system is the most widely used system, the Allen-Mills has long been used in academic publications, and will be used throughout the rest of this article, unless otherwise noted.

Consonants

Nankôre has a small inventory of consonants, only thirteen in number. Noteworthy is the paucity of voiced consonants which contributes to the limited number of consonants in the Nankôre language. The following table contains the entire consonantal inventory:

Bilabial Dental Alveolar Apical Post-alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
Nasal m n
Stop p t k ʔ
Affricate t͡ʃ
Fricative s ʃ ɦ
Approximant j
Flap ɾ

Vowels

  Front Near- front Central Near- back Back
Close
Blank vowel trapezoid.svg
i
u
ɪ
e
o
ɛ
a
  Near-close
Close-mid
Mid
Open-mid
Near-open
Open

Prosody

Stress

Phonotactics

Morphophonology

Morphology

Nouns

Number

Nouns do not inflect for number, which is usually determined by context, but a numeral joined by the Connective si= to its head noun can be used to indicate plurality, eg:

ôhi sikurasno.
/'o:ɦi ʃi ku'raʃno/
ôhi si=kurasno
two CONN girl

two girls

If the noun is a core argument, then the auxiliary obligatorily marks for number. Number is indicated on the auxiliary by the affix -n-. For plural Agents, it is infixed, and for plural Patients, it is suffixed. Additionally, if the auxiliary is marked with the Inverse prefix, the prefix redundantly encodes number by vowel length, eg ta- for the singular, and tā= for the plural. Some examples are given below:

Example of a HA singular Agent acting on a LA singular Patient:

kurasno rompóy rayrokor nitá'.
/ku'raʃno rom'poɪ 'raɪrokor nɪ'taʔ/
kurasno rompóy Ø-rayro=kor Ø-ni-Ø-itá-ʔ
girl.HA dog.LA DIR=play=EVID.HEAR DIR-PST-HS-COP-LS

I heard that the girl played with the dog recently.

Example of a HA plural Agent acting on a LA singular Patient:

kurasno rompóy rayrokor nintá'.
/ku'raʃno rom'poɪ 'raɪrokor nɪn'taʔ/
kurasno rompóy Ø-rayro-kor Ø-ni-n-itá-'
girl.HA dog.LA play-EVID.HEAR DIR-PST-HP-COP-LS

I heard that the girls played with the dog recently.

Example of a LA singular Agent acting on a HA plural Patient:

rompóy kurasno tarayrokor tānintá'.
/romp'oɪ ku'raʃno ta'raɪrokor 'ta:nɪntáʔ/
rompóy kurasno ta=rayro=kor tā-ni-n-itá-ʔ
dog.LA girl.HA INV=play-HEAR INV.LS-PST-HP-COP-LS

I heard that the dog played with the girls recently.

Example of a LA plural Agent acting on a HA plural Patient:

rompóy kurasno tarayrokor tānintan.
/romp'oɪ ku'raʃno ta'raɪrokor 'ta:nɪntan/
rompóy kurasno ta=rayro=kor tā-ni-n-itá-n
dog.LA girl.HA INV=play=EVID.HEAR INV.PL-PST-HP-COP-LP

I heard that the dogs played with the girls recently.


The Nominal Hierarchy

Nankôre nouns, at first glance, appears very simple. They are not inflected for case, number, or gender, nor do adpositions indicate their directional or positional relationships. Number is distinguished only in pronouns. Nevertheless, the nominal system has a complex hierarchy of animacy, which although unmarked, is a semantic feature of the noun and pronoun. This hierarchy of animacy is used to determine the syntactic roles of the core arguments of a clause, namely that of Agent and Patient. Arguments that are higher in the Animacy Hierarchy are by default considered Agents, whereas lower animacy arguments are considered Patients. If the syntactic roles of two core arguments are reversed, namely a lower animacy argument acts as an Agent on a higher animacy Patient argument, a special construction known as the Inverse Voice, involving the prefix tā-/ta-, is added to both the first verb in the verb phrase and the auxiliary verb itá'. Thus, the Inverse Voice is doubly-marked.

Where a given NP falls within the animacy hierarchy must be memorized in order to use the verbal inverse prefix tā-/ta- correctly (see section on Verbs for additional details regarding inverse marking).

The Nankôre nominal hierarchy has been traditionally classified along two axes, the first marking the noun class, and the second marking levels of hierarchy within a given class. Animacy along classes decrease as one traverses from Supernatural/Phenomonological onwards towards the Ephemeral. Animacy decreases as one goes down the Level axis. The following table illustrates the animacy relations between the noun hierarchies, although in reality, animacy is more complex than the table would suggest, as discourse and other contextual elements may influence a native speaker's expression of the hierarchy in a given utterance.


Table of Nominal Animacy hierarchy
Noun Class
Level Supernatural &
Phenomonological
Human Animals Lower Animals Riverine Weather Plants Geophysical Material Ephemeral
Mammals Birds
1 Gods, the Earth, the Sun, the Sky/Heavens Adults >> Children predator >> prey Reptiles (excluding snakes) Rushing rivers Clouds Forests Mountains Liquid, usually in a container Magic
2 Demons Older >> Younger wild >> domesticated Slow-moving rivers Rain Snakes Large Trees Glaciers Food Actions
3 Human & Animal Spirits Male >> Female Pet (usually carnivorous) >> Non-Pet (usually raised for food) Amphibians Streams
Brooks
Springs
Snow Small Trees Cliffs
Jagged outcroppings
Tumuli
Boulders
Fabrics
Fibers
Skins
Clothing
Emotions
4 Destructive Forces of Nature Large >> Small Fish Lakes Wind Fruit/Nut Trees Flat land
Gently rolling landscape
Plains
Wood Thoughts
5 Lightning Strong >> Weak Breeze Crustaceans Bushes
Shrubs
Buildings Disease
6 Ocean Quick >> Slow Insects Moving Water Prairie grass Metals States
7 Thunderstorms Intelligent >> Unintelligent Carnivore >> Herbivore >> Carrion Eater Worms Air Leafy plants Jewels and gemstones Abstractions
8 Fire Fertile >> Infertile Stud >> Gelded Flight >> Flightless Mollusks Muggy weather Mushrooms
Fungi
Mosses
Gelatinous sea invertebrates
Stones, rocks, pebbles Death


If two nouns with equal animacy but different number (obtained either through pronominal referents or context) appear as core arguments, the plural noun is assigned a higher animacy status than the singular noun. However, if animacy and number are equal, the most recently introduced noun, providing it was introduced as a core argument, is assigned the higher animacy status.

In situations were two nouns have equal animacy and equal number, the rules for assigning the position each core argument in the hierarchy is as follows:

  1. If one of the nouns is newly introduced information, it is considered less animate because the older noun has been presumably been the center of discourse and at least during part of the previous discourse it was agent. SOV order is usually used, particularly if pragmatics and other contextual cues cannot disambiguate the syntactic roles
  2. HOWEVER, if the new noun is topicalized with the clitic =si, it is considered the agent.
  3. Another pattern, where a new noun is introduced as a Patient argument, it is regarded as the less animate argument once the older noun is reintroduced back into the discourse in a transitive construction.
  4. If the animacy level of each argument cannot be resolved, then the unmarked SOV word order is used when context can't disambiguate syntactic roles.

Derivational

  • -no: Diminutive suffix, e.g. kuras (woman), kurasno (girl)
  • -CV(C) reduplication: Augmentive, e.g. kurasras (big woman); analogous to Minhast augmentation
  • -rV-CV(C) infixation + reduplication: Augmentive-Deprecative: nahón -> nannarohono, from na~na~ro-hón-ho (big/fat ugly man); similar to Minhast -ra- infixation and reduplication of first syllable, e.g. Minhast kas~ka~ra-slub "large dog, hunting hound" < kaslub "dog"

Adjectival Suffixes

Nankôre lacks a separate word class for adjectives. They may be expressed as attributive verbs, e.g. Aryak neshkak itá, "Aryak is smart", Kanko shoykar sip'itá', "Kanko (an indigenous vegetable) makes good" (for your health), or a suffix may be attached to the noun it modifies. These suffixes are called adjectival suffixes. Some of these suffixes are etymologically derived from an attributive verb, e.g. karyak itá > -kerek >, as in Kurasnokerek "the girl is red" (i.e. the girl is blushing). Others appear to be derived from separate roots, c.f. "Aryak-arku", ("clever Aryak") vs "Aryak neshkak itá" ("Aryak is clever"). Theoretically an unlimited number of suffixes may be appended to the noun, but in practice the number of adjectival suffixes seldom exceeds three. The suffixes may appear in any order, so long as the meaning is comprehensible. Scope ordering determines the position of the suffixes relative to each other, with those of wider scope appearing towards the end, as in the following example:

Kenenkorpitasnune iná'.
/kɛnɛnkorpɪtaʃ'nunɛ ɪ'naʔ/
kenenkor-pita-snu-ne iná
bed-plush-red-soft-very COP

It is a very soft, red and plush bed.

While the number of adjectival suffixes are considerable, they remain for the most part a closed class. Moreover, with a few exceptions, each adjectival suffix may be expressed by a corresponding stative verb.

Hosiari sikenenkorpitasnune iná torocak ta'itāhi.
/ho'ʃari sɪkɛnɛnkorpɪtaʃ'nunɛ ɪ'naʔ torot͡ʃak taʔɪt'a:hi/
hosiari si-kenenkor-pita-snu-ne iná ta-horoci=ak ta-itá'-hi
PROX.DEM CONN-bed-plush-red-soft-very COP INV-sleep=1S+3S INV-AUX-CAUS

This very soft, red and plush bed makes me sleep (well).


There are seven observed categories of adjectival suffixes:

  1. Quality
  2. Quantity
  3. Strength
  4. Size
  5. Color
  6. Tactile
  7. Other Attributes

Demonstratives and Deictics

Spatial
Type Independent Clitic Translation Example Nahenic Cognates
Proximal hosiairi
/ho'ʃjaɪɾi/
=skoro
  • this one near me
  • now
  • hosiari sinán
    nan=iskoro
    "this man here"
  • oriyat=hepi "today"
  • Minhast Gull Speaker dialect:
    -nussar-
  • Nahónda: nótsalo
Medio-Proximal yaiyairi =hori
=ori
  • that one by you
  • just awhile ago
  • yaiyairi sinán
    nan=hori
    "that man next to you"
  • Minhast Gull Speaker dialect:
    -eyyar-, -yyar-
  • Nahónda: iyáyalo
Distal paipairi =nko
=kekori
  • yonder, that one near him/her/them
  • yesterday, a few days ago
  • paipairi sinán
    nan=inko "yonder man"
  • Minhast Gull Speaker dialect:
    -ppeyyar-
  • Nahónda: pʼáyalo
Invisible rosiairi
/ɾoʃ'jaɪɾi/

=nanak

  • that one beyond the horizon,
    or occluded by a distant object
  • rosiairi sinán
    nan=nanak
    "that man
    (e.g. on the other side
    of the mountain)"
  • Minhast Gull Speaker dialect:
    -ruššar-


Temporal

The Temporal Deitic particles usually occur at the head of a sentence, e.g. Antak Anut Amerika san-kamuk hitá' , "A year ago, Anut flew to America". In multiclausal sentences, the clause in which they appear have scope over the all other clauses following it. At least one, yorai, cannot appear after the initial clause. Some particles also have a clitic form which can attach to the main verb, as in Anut Amerika san-kamuk-entak hitá'.

type Independent Clitic Meaning Example Nahenic Cognates
Past yoráy =yoráy Once upon a time; back then Example
inkát =(i)nkát Several years ago Example
šeyhori =šeyhor A few years ago Example
antak =(e)ntak A year ago Example
kake =(e)kak Several days ago, last week Example
nateha =nate The day before yesterday Example
kirá =kira Yesterday Example
Diurnal yohori =ho Earlier this morning Example
tantori =tora Just awhile ago Example
yot =(e)yót Now Example
nas =(e)nas Soon Example
Future kesór =k(e)sor Tomorrow Example
nekós =n(i)kos In the next few days Example
nankóy =n(a)koy In the indeterminate future Example

Quantifiers

Quantifier Substantive Attributive
All kamun kamun si=
Most toman toman si=
Some cene cisi=
cis=
Many eco ecisi=
eci=
ec=
Both hani hanisi=
hansi=
has=
Each nune nisi=
nis=
Few cato caci=
cac=
Another/Other hane hanesi=
hanse=

Interrogatives

Header text Header text Notes
Who ra', rā Possibly cognate with Minhast redad
What pôh c.f. Minhast bak
When tu'a
Where nahí' c.f. Minhast nakki. Also, note dialectal variant enket (Sapshira dialect)
Why penóh
Which yôri- C.f. yôhori, to choose, Minhast yāhuran (to make a turn at an intersection of a path, road)
Polarity aní Used for yes-no questions, occurs in sentence-final position.

Cognate with Minhast ni (Classical, Horse Speaker, and Salmonic dialects)

=yo Like ani, used for yes-no questions. However, it cliticizes to the first word of the clause.


The Interrogatives manifest WH-movement, appearing immediately before the auxiliary itá' with rising intonation and a slight pause after the main VP. The Irrealis suffix -hi often accompanies WH-words but is often dropped, as in the second example, especially among younger speakers. However, it is required for polar questions, i.e. questions marked with aní, in which case its allmorph -ha is used.

Okep nari ayrarke, penóh hô'itáhi?
'okɛp 'na:ɾi aɪ'ɾaɾke pe'noɦ ɦo:ʔɪ'taɦi
Okep nari ayrarke, penóh Ø-hô-Ø-'itá-ʔ-hi
PN child spank why DIR-HOD-HS-COP-LS-IRR

Why did Okep spank (her) child earlier this morning?
Kā tahône, ra' tasip'itá'?
ka: ta'ɦo:nɛ ɾaʔ taʃɪpʔ'ɪta
kā ta=hone, ra' ta-sip-Ø-itá-ʔ
2S INV.SG=cry who INV-CAUS-HS-COP-LS

Who is making you cry?

The following two sentences are polarity questions, with no difference in meaning. The third sentence, containing both polarity markers =yo and sentence-final particle aní, implies incredulity:

Koykâre cori yoskara sip'itáha' aní?
/koɪ'ka:ɾɛ 'tʃoɾi 'joʃkaɾa ʃɪpʔɪtaɦa a'ni/
koykâre cori yoskara sip-Ø-itá-ʔ-hi aní
boy house burn CAUS-DIR.PRS.HS-COP-LS-IRR Q

Did the boy burn the house (down)?
Koykâryo cori yoskara sip'itáhi?
/koɪ'ka:ɾjo 'tʃoɾi 'joʃkaɾa ʃɪpʔɪ'taɦi/
koykâre=yo cori yoskara sip-Ø-itá-ʔ-hi
boy=Q house burn CAUS-DIR.PRS.HS-COP-LS-IRR

Did the boy burn the house (down)?
Koykaryo cori yoskara sip'itáha aní?
/koɪ'kaɾjo 'tʃoɾi 'joʃkaɾa ʃɪpʔɪ'taɦa 'ani/
koykâre=yo cori yoskara sip-Ø-itá-ʔ-hi ani
boy=Q house burn CAUS-DIR.PRS.HS-COP-LS-IRR Q

Did the boy burn the house (down)?

With the exception of the polarity particle aní, interrogatives can also serve as conjunctions, provided that the interrogatives is joined to the dependent clause with the connective si=:

Horyák kosno nitá' penóh si-Okep nari ayrarkekór ho'itá'.
hôri=ák kosno Ø-n-Ø-itá-ʔ penóh si=Okep nari ayrarke-kór Ø-ho-Ø-'itá-ʔ
NEG=1S hear DIR-PST-HS.COP-LS why CONN=PN nephew spank-HEAR DIR-HOD-HS-COP-LS

I did not hear/receive the news why Okep spanked (her) nephew early this morning.

Neither a pseudo-incorporated noun or adjunct may appear between the Interrogative and the auxiliary. The next sentence is well-formed, with the pseudo-incorporated noun appearing before the interrogative penóh (why), whereas the next sentence is unfelicitous, since PINs are barred from appearing between the WH-word and the auxiliary:

Koykâre yoskara cori penóh itá'?
koykâre yoskara cori penóh Ø-itá-ʔ
boy burn house why DIR.PRS.HS-COP-LS

Why did the boy burn the house (down)?
*Koykâre yoskara penóh cori sip'itá'?
koykâre yoskara penóh cori sip-Ø-itá-ʔ
boy house burn why CAUS-DIR.PRS.HS-COP-LS

Why did the boy burn the house (down)?

Interrogatives can also serves as Conditionals by a =si Topicalization structure:

Ra'si karámpo taneknek ta'itá', siakos tamáko itá'
'raʔʃi ka'ɾampo ta'nɛknɛk 'taʔɪtaʔ 'ʃakoʃ ta'mako ʔɪ'taʔ
Ra'=si karampo ta=neknek ta-Ø-itá-Ø, si=akos tamakó Ø-Ø-itá-ʔ
Who=TOP bear INV=kill INV-HS-COP-LS CONN=1P danger.liability DIR-HS-COP-LS

Whoever slaughtered the bear (in this manner) is a danger to us all.

The Topic marker =si should not be confused with the Connective si= that is linking the embedded clause to the matrix clause. The former is an enclitic, whereas the latter is a proclitic.

Among the Interrogative pronouns, ra' ranks the highest in animacy; nevertheless, Interrogative pronouns are lower than non-Interrogative nouns in the animacy hierarchy.

Yôri- is always prefixed to the noun it modifies and can never appear as an independent word. It can be prefixed to pôh and ra' , i.e. yôripoh and yôra' where it takes the meaning "which one", yôripoh for Noun Classes II and III, the sentient members of Class I, and yôra' for the rest.


Pronouns

Nankôre pronouns distinguish animacy, number, and transitivity. The transitive forms are portmanteaus, which in combination with the presence or absence of the verb's inverse marker, encodes both the agent and patient.

Animacy in Nankôre is arranged in the following hierarchy:

Pronominal hierarchy
1st > 2nd > 3rd > 4th

Plurality generally does not affect the animacy level across persons. However, if two 3rd person arguments have the same animacy and one of the arguments is plural, then the plural argument is assigned a higher animacy level.

The paradigm for the transitive pronouns are as follows:

Transitive Portmanteau Pronouns
Singular Plural
1S 2S 3S 4S 1P.INC 1P.EXCL 2P 3P 4P
1S -- ekká' ekenko enkár -- -- kamme enka nerá
2S ekká'

ka
-- kanko

ka
korí kanná kanné -- kanenko kâre
3S ekenko kanko

ka
-- enkará enko nenko kanenko -- kankór
4S enkár korí enkará -- ánkare nénkare kánnare eranka --
Plural x Plural
1P.INCL -- -- ánkame ananka ánnahi
1P.EXCL -- -- nékkame nenka énnahi
2P ánkame nékkame -- nekákkame nákkame
3P ananka nenka nekákkame -- nékkahi
4P ánnahi énnahi nákkame nékkahi --

Transitive verbs take two NPs as core arguments. The case relations of the two arguments are determined by the position of the noun in the animacy hierarchy; the core argument that is higher in the animacy hierarchy is assigned the Agent role, and the other argument is assigned the Patient role. However, an inverse affix tā=/tāh=/ta=/t= is prefixed to the primary verb, as well as a redundant ta- affix that is added to the auxiliary to alter the argument structure and promote the lower-animacy argument to Agent role.

The intransitive forms consist of both independent and cliticized forms, as illustrated in the following table:

Intransitive Pronouns
Person Singular Plural
Independent Clitic Long Form Clitic
1 eyak =yak
=ak
aná =na
2 eká =ka kamme =kamme
3 enko =ko
=na
enká =enká
=nka
=ka
4 erá =ra nami =nami

If used as a core argument, the intransitive singular forms and the plural short forms may appear just before the clause-final auxiliary itá,e.g. Nitori kā 'itá! (You are a fool); or the clitic form of the pronoun attaches to the main verb, e.g. Nitoriká 'itá!. They may also be used to emphasize a noun, in which case the pronoun precedes the noun linked with the connective si=, as in tā si=naho (She/Her, the mother).

As earlier stated, nouns in Nankôre are not inflected, and the same is true for pronouns. Moreover, oblique pronominal arguments cannot be inserted into the verb phrase, but instead must appear before it. If oblique arguments appear outside their clause, particularly in topic-comment structures, they must be joined to it with the connective clitic si=. Context alone determines the semantic role of the oblique. To illustrate, the sentence Maska si=ohipna koykare ekán itá (anthill=CONN twig boy twist COP.PST), i.e. "The boy inserted the twig into the anthill", the oblique noun maska (anthill is marked with the clitic si= to join it to the rest of the sentence.

Verbs

The verb phrase in Nankôre requires at a minimum an initial verb, plus one of the clause-final auxiliaries, 'itá' for transitive clauses or intransitive clauses where the subject's theta-role is that of Agent, and 'iná ' for all other intransitive clauses. A sort of division of labor exists between these components; one set of clitics or affixes may attach to one of the components, and another set to the other component.

The following set of markers cliticize to the first verb of the VP, which may be either the main verb, or a coverb. All preverbal clitics attach to the the initial verb:

  1. Inverse
  2. Aspect

However, a set of bound morphemes, occurring as suffixes, attach exclusively to the main verb, regardless of whether the verb occurs initial position in the verb phrase or not:

  1. Evidentials
Evidentials Meaning Example
-kor hearsay Example
Example Example Example
Example Example Example

Modal verbs, when they occur, follow the main verb, displaying leftward scope-ordering, e.g. Kôreak képorih iná'! "I am trying to speak!".

Finally, the auxiliary 'iná'/'itá' hosts the following set of affixes:

  1. Inverse
  2. Tense
  3. Number
  4. Voice
  5. Irrealis


The Verb Phrase Structure

The following example demonstrates a complex verb predicate, with the Inverse marker ta= cliticizing to the coverb yam (accompany) in the Imperfect aspect, followed by the main verb yayak (go up a mountain) with the Hearsay Evidential suffix -kor, and finally the auxiliary marked with the redundent Inverse clitic ta'=.

Rompóy kurasno tayampo yayak hosmakór ta'nitá'.
/rom'poɪ ku'raʃno ta'yampo 'yayak hoʃma'kor taʔnɪt'aʔ/
Rompóy kurasno ta-yam-po yayak hosma-kor ta'-∅-nitá-ʔ.
dog.LA girl.HA INV-accompany-IMPF go.up.mountain-EVID.HEAR INV-HS-PST.COP-LS

The dog was going up the mountain with the girl, I heard.

The verbal elements follow a relatively strict order. The required elements for a VP to be well-formed are the Main Verb and the Auxiliary. Adjuncts are unbound morphemes, and exhibit rather free order; however, they are barred from appearing between the Inverse Marker and any following verb, be it the Coverb or the Main Verb. If a Coverb appears between the Inverse Marker and Main Verb, an adjunct may appear between the Coverb and Main Verb. However, if the Inverse Marker is not followed by a Coverb, then an adjunct may not appear between the Inverse Marker and the Main Verb. Moreover, if Pseudo-Noun Incorporation (PNI) takes place, an adjunct may not intervene between the Main Verb and the Pseudo-incorporated noun (PIN).

Although Nankôre is not a polysynthetic language, templatic representations like those of its polysynthetic cousins Minhast and Nahónda, provide a useful visual tool to demonstrate the Nankôre verb phrase structure:

Verb Conjugation Table
Position Type Subtype Realization
-3 Inverse Marker 1 ta=
-2 Coverbs
Directionals/Positionals
Modals
1 Aspect x Examplexxx
0 Primary Verb
+1 Quasi-Incorporated Noun Examples
+2 Auxiliary Inverse Marker 2 ta-
+3 Inchoative
+4 Causative
+5 Irrealis

Coverbs

Coverbs in Nankôre formal grammar are auxiliary verbs that serve to provide additional syntactic information to the main verb itself, or to the core arguments. Although several types of coverbs exist, the Directionals-Positionals coverbs are the most prominent. Directional and positional coverbs serve as a way of indicating case relations of the core arguments, somewhat like Applicatives in other languages, such as those of its distant cousin Minhast. In fact the older linguistic literature often refers to these coverbs as "Applicative Verbs". The Directionals-Positionals coverbs always precede all other modal verbs, which are scope-ordered.

Directional-Positional Coverbs

Directional and positional coverbs have both a long and short form. The long form, also called the Independent Form, may serve as a bona fide standalone verb, or it may serve as a coverb provided that no clitics attach to it. Thus the coverb sanha, in its long form, means "to go towards, to approach" as an independent verb, but its short form, san, always indicates motion towards an entity; this sort of syntactic relation is usually handled by the Allative case in languages which indicate case marking on the noun. Clitics that otherwise attach to the main verb, such as the Inverse marker and and aspect markers, move to and attach to the first occurring coverb of the VP. Clitics attach only to the short form. Additionally, morphophonemic alternations may take place when the Inverse marker ta= cliticizes to the coverb, such as in the Subessive cor-.


Goal Coverb Forms
Long Short Coverb + Inverse
Goal sanha san tasan


Anut America sanha kamukkor itáʔ.
anut america sanha kamuk-kor itáʔ
PN.HA PN.LA going.towards fly-EVID.HEAR COP.DIR

Anut supposedly flew to America.
Rompóy sikatti yacor pitasi san yapokor itáʔ.
rompóy si=katti yacor san yapo-kor itáʔ
dog CONN=PN.HA ball.LA going.towards throw-EVID.HEAR COP.DIR

Kathy tossed the ball towards the dog.
Katti yacor san yapokor rompóy itáʔ.
Katti yacor san yapokor rompóy itáʔ
PN.HA ball.LA going.towards throw-EVID.HEAR dog COP.DIR

Kathy tossed the ball towards the dog.
Kurasno siyacor rompóy tasan nahoyra ayorkor ta'itáʔ.
Kurasno siyacor rompóy ta-san nahoyra ayor-kor ta-ʔitáʔ
girl CONN=ball.LA dog.HA INV-going.towards do.repeatedly bring-EVID.HEAR dog INV-COP

The dog repeatedly brought back the ball to the girl.
  • Benefactive


Directional and Positional Coverbs
Long Short Coverb + Inverse Sample Sentence Translation Comment
Goal sanha san tasan Anut America sanha kamuk-kor itá. Anut supposedly flew to America. -kor = HEARSAY.EVID, is suffixed to the main verb.
Dative tohta toht tatóh Anut nanna tatóh ittáh tanno ta'itá. Anut gave his mother water. c.f Minhast dative applicative -dut-, menā' "mother", hittu "give", dannua "water"
Benefactive nasko nas tanas Nanhoska karen tā-nas eynakor itá. It is said the (sacred) tree gave (life) for the people. c.f. Minhast postposition =nī, =ni
Comitative yampe yam tam Anut yam temenkor itá Anut walked beside the river with him. temen = to perform an activity by a river
Instrumental makôr mak tamak Ehok mak-nahoyra yurasnanetkor itá. The adolescent struck (it) repeatedly with a club.
Ablative risa ris tars Šôni Nan Kamun Koryas ris mankor itá First Man emerged from (out of) the snow. c.f. Minhast applicative -raħk-
Šôni Nan = "the First Man"
Kamun Koryas "All Snow" (the birthplace of First Man)
Locative neyhi ne tan Yonka asrok ta-n akunkor hô'itá. I heard that a fly buzzed inside Yonka('s mouth) this morning. asrok = fly, insect
Vialis nahke nak, nahk tanak, tanahk Ka pohak ta-nahke šokor-nui itá' I saw the river take you through the cave. c.f. Minhast applicative -naħk-, -ni-
pohak = cave
šokor = to flow
-nui- = VIS.EVID
Prolative roskar ros taros Ak piri roskar hô'itá'. I crossed the street earlier this morning piri = street, road, path
Superessive oros or tor Ayôhe arupenrô or yaunkekor hô'itá. The airplane flew over Ayôhe township earlier this morning. arupenrô = airplane
yaunke = to fly
Subessive yorha yor toyor, cor Ekurki saska yor-ro kahorokor itá. The turtle swam underneath the ice. ekurki = turtle
saska = ice
kahoro = to swim
Ablative-Superessive isuk suk, su task, tasku (Example) (Example)
Ablative-Subessive royna roy taroy (Example) (Example)
Riverine temen tem tatem No si-kurasno tem rohnaskor itá. The girls supposedly were playing by the river.
Montaigne yayak yay tay Kahno côri yay cire/ciri itá' The old man went up the mountain.

Aspect

The verb, in contrast to the auxiliary, encodes aspect with clitics that attach to the end of the word. If a coverb appears, the aspect marker will cliticize to the coverb, otherwise it will cliticize to the main verb. Nankôre possesses several aspect markers, as illustrated in the following table:

Aspect Marker Meaning
Perfect
Semelfactive =no completed in one single motion/act, e.g. hit
Simple Imperfect =ro
Durative/Repetitive =nahoyra, =noyra several times
Habitative =(h)osma, =sma often
Factive =yosir (general truth statement, e.g. "The sun rises in the east")
Continuative =namporo to continue
Cessative =ciri to stop
Terminative =ruykáno to finish
Incipient =kara about to
Approximative =tontoro almost, nearly
Frequentative =nokori, -(o)nkori often

Main Verb

The main verb is believed to be descended from a verbal noun and carries the main semantic meaning of the verb phrase. The main verb hosts the Inverse Marker tā-/ta-, aspect, and evidential/modal markers. As mentioned earlier, the Inverse and aspect markers may detach from the main verb and attach to a converb, while the modal/evidential suffixes remain tightly bound to the main verb.

Moods and Evidentials

The Mood and Evidential markers occur as bound suffixes of the main verb. Unlike the Inverse and aspect markers, they do not detach from the main verb but remain bound to the main verb.

Evidentials
Neutral -∅
Emphatic-Factual -mas
-masne
Hearsay -kor
Visual -nui
Inferential -yay
Intentive -neat, -ne
Dubitative -anene
Modals

Modal verbs follow the main verb. Modals include a wide range of expressions, such as ability, potentiality, similitude, desire, inclination, etc.

Modal Meaning Nahenic Cognates
Potential makán be able to, can
Necessitive nehái need to
Desiderative tasmi want to
Conative si
s=
to try to, attempt Cognate with Minhast sunep- (to push)
Inceptive yári begin to
Resumptive kenóy again

Examples of usage of the modal verbs follow:

Ekenko tuas sitá', yankór[2] seskoro sioykar ináka.
Ekenko tuaʃ 'ʃi'taʔ yan'koɾ 'ʃɛʃkoɾo 'ʃoɪkaɾ ɪ'naka
ekenko tuas s-itá' yankór se=skoro sioykar iná-ka.
1S.3S wash.out CON-AUX.TRN however reed=DEM.PROX good be-NEG.

I'm trying to wash (the stain) out, but this reed brush isn't good.

The Auxiliary

In terms of morphological complexity, the auxiliary hosts more affixes and clitics than the main verb; in addition to redundantly marking the Inverse voice, it also encodes tense, number, irrealis, inchoativity, and causation through a complex series of affixation.

The auxiliary serves two purposes:

  • To links noun phrases to their predicates:
Nan konkekma yoriká ináhi?
/nan kon'kekma 'yo:riká ɪn'ahi/
nan konkekma, yôri-ká ∅-iná-ʔ-hi
human snake which-2S DIR-PRS.COP.STAT-S-IRR

Are you man or snake?
  • To serve as a host for tense, number, animacy, and voice markers to the verb phrase.
Kanro kacekák hôsipnitá'.
/'kanro ka't͡ʃekák ho:sipnɪ'taʔ/
kanro kacek=ák ∅-hô-sip-nitá-ʔ
old.man hide=1S DIR-HOD-HS-PST.COP.TRN-LS

I hid the old man yesterday.
Ea? Kacek ka, yôrikani hôsipnitáhi?
/ea 'kat͡ʃek ka, yo:ri'kani ho:ʃɪpni'tahi/
ea kacek ka yôri-kani ∅-hô-sip-nitá-ʔ-hi
MIR hide 2S which-place DIR-HOD-CAUS-HS-PST.COP.TR-LS-IRR

Really? Where did you hide him?

The Inverse marker ta'- indicates a lower animate NP is the Agent acting on a higher animate NP whose role is that of Patient. The marker occurs in two locations: as a clitic to the first verb of the VP, and as a bound prefix to the auxiliary itá':

kattí tarayrokor ta'itá'.
/ka't:i ta'raɪrokor taʔɪ'taʔ/
kattí ta-rayro-kor ta-itá-ʔ
2S+3S INV-play-HEAR INV.LS-PST-COP-LS

I heard that he played with you recently.

Number is marked on both core arguments: for transitive clauses, singular HA arguments are null-marked, and LA arguments are marked with the suffix . For plurality, plural HA arguments are marked with a prefixed or infixed -n-, while the LA argument is marked with the suffix -n for plurality. Additionally, the vowel of the Inverse marker lengthens when the LA argument is plural; for example, the singular LA argument of the present tense auxiliary is ta'ita', whereas the plural form of the verb is tā'inta'. The Hodiernal tense is an exception: the vowel of the Inverse marker neither undergoes vowel lengthening, nor does it receive compensatory stress; the form *tāhô'intá' is ungrammatical, the form tahô'intá' instead. This is presumably because the vowel of the tense marker, -ô-, suppresses lengthening of a preceding vowel according to Nankôre phonological rules, thus moving the stress rightward.

The Inchoative consists of a the prefix yis-, which frequently occurs as a circumfix, y- + -s-, for certain conjugations. The affix causes the Inverse affix ta(h)- to palatize; the -s- segment of the circumfixal form may occur before or after a pluralizing -n- affix, or the tense-bearing prefix of a given conjugation. Knowing which form of the Inchoative, and where the -s- segment occurs is unpredictable and requires memorization. When used in transitive sentences, it serves to mark the Inceptive aspect. The -s- component has been shown to be cognate with the Minhast inchoative affix -saxt-.

The Causative is marked with the affixes sip- or -sp-, which is cognate with the Causatives in Minhast -šp- and Nahónda -ps-. As Causatives typically increase the valency in a clause's argument structure, the Causee is typically the Patient argument, and the target of the Causee is a si- marked argument phrase that follows the matrix clause; a slight pause usually occurs before the si- connective. The construction suggest the Causee's target is an anti-topic, although it may be fronted at the beginning of its governing clause by the si- connective for topicalization.

The Irrealis -hi is suffixed to the auxiliary. It is obligatory for all Future tenses. It also surfaces in imperatives, yes-no questions which are marked with the =yo marker cliticizing to the first or second word of a sentence, as in Cire=yo tā'itá'-hi? "Did he die?", and in WH-questions when the event actually happening has not yet been established as having occurred; for example, Enket cire tā'itá'?, "Where did he die?" implies that the person's death is a fact, and it is only the location that is being inquired, versus Enket cire tā'itá'-hi? implies that not only is the location unknown, but the person's actual death happening still remains to be established. An interesting structure, called the "double interrogative", is a combination of a WH-word followed by a word bearing the =yo clitic, which also turns the question into yes-no question: Enket cire=yo tā'itá'-hi?, "Did he die, and if so, where?" The -hi suffix is believed to be cognate with the Minhast Irrealis clitic .

Transitive Paradigm
 Present Low Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
High Animate Agent SG itá' itá'ita' yistá' yistá'ita sip'itá' (pis'itá') sip'itá'ita (pis'itá'ita')
PL nitá' nitá'ita' yisnitá' yisnitá'ita' sipnitá' (pisnitá') sipnitá'ita' (pisnitá'ita')
High Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
Low Animate Agent SG ta’itá' ta’itá'ita castá' castá'ita taspitá' taspitá'ita
PL tānitá' tānitá'ita casnitá' casnitá'ita tāsipnitá' tāsipnitá'ita
Hodiernal Past ‡
Low Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
High Animate Agent SG hô'itá' hô'itá'ita yôsitá' yôsitá'ita hôsipnitá' hôsipnitá'ita
PL hô'intá' hô'intá'ita yôsintá' yôsintá'ita hôsipnintá' hôsipnintá'ita
High Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
Low Animate Agent SG tahô'itá' tahô'itá'ita costá' costá'ita tāhópsintá' tāhópsintá'ita
PL tahô'intá' tahô'intá'ita cosnitá' cosnitá'ita tāhópsintá' tāhópsintá'ita
Past
Low Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
High Animate Agent SG nitá' nitá'ita yisnitá' yisnitá'ita sipnitá' sipnitá'ita
PL nintá' nintá'ita yisnitá' yisnitá'ita sipnintá' sipnintá'ita
High Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
Low Animate Agent SG ta’nitá' ta’nitá'ita casnitá' casnitá'ita tasipnitá' tasipnitá'ita
PL tānintá' tānintá'ita casnintá' casnintá'ita tāsipnintá' tāsipnintá'ita
Medio-Distal Past
Low Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
High Animate Agent SG hitá' hitá'ita yistá' yistá'ita hispitá' hisiptá'ita
PL hintá' hintá'ita yisintá' yisintá'ita hisipnitá' hisipnitá'ita
High Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
Low Animate Agent SG tahitá' tahitá'ita castá' castá'ita tasiphitá' tasiphitá'ita
PL tāhintá' tāhintá'ita casintá' casinta'ita tāsiphintá' tāsiphintá'ita
Distal Past
Low Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
High Animate Agent SG pitá' pitá'ita yipsitá' yipsitá'ita pisiptá' pisipta'ita
PL pintá' pintá'ita yipsintá' yipsintá'ita pisipnitá pisipnitá'ita
High Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
Low Animate Agent SG ta’pitá' ta’pitá'ita caspitá casipnitá'ita tápsipnitá' tápsipnitá'ita
PL tápsintá' tápsintá'ita casipnitá casipnitá'ita tápsipnitá' tápsipnitá'ita
Medio-Proximal Future
Low Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
High Animate Agent SG nitáh nitahá'ita yisnitáh yisnitahá'ita nisiptáh nisiptahá'ita
PL nintáh nintahá'ita yisnitahá yisnitahá'ita nisipnitáh nisipnitahá'ita
High Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
Low Animate Agent SG ta’nitáh tantahá'ita casnitáh casnitahá'ita tasipnitáh tasipnitahá'ita
PL tānintáh tānintahá'ita casnintáh casnintáha'ita tāsipnitáh tāsipnintahá'ita
Immediate Future
Low Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
High Animate Agent SG hitáh hitahá'ita yistáh yistahá'ita hisipitáh hisiptahá'ita
PL hintáh hintahá'ita yisnitáh yisnitahá'ita hispintáh hisipnitahá'ita
High Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
Low Animate Agent SG tahitáh tahitahá'ita castáh castáh tahisipitáh tahisiptahá'ita
PL tāhintáh tāhintahá'ita cahsintáh cahsintahá'ita tāhispintáh tāhisipnitahá'ita
Distal Future
Low Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
High Animate Agent SG pitáh pitahá'ita yipsitáh yipsitahá'ita pisiptáh pisiptahá'ita
PL pintáh pintahá'ita yipsiptáh yipsiptahá'ita pisipnitáh pisipnitahá'ita
High Animate Patient
Indicative Inchoative Causative
SG PL SG PL SG PL
High Animate Agent SG ta’pitáh taptahá'ita capsitáh capsitaháh tapsiptáh tapsiptahá'ita
PL tāpintáh tāpintahá'ita capsintáh capsintahá'ita tápsipnitáh tápsipnitahá'ita

‡ Note the irregular Indicative Hodiernal tense form; the Inverse marker does not lengthen as it does for the rest of the paradigm.

Intransitive-Active Paradigm

The paradigm for the itá auxiliary with intransitive verbs is not as elaborate as its transitive counterpart, nevertheless it still displays a great deal of complexity. As in the transitive paradigm, an Agent is required as a core argument, but unlike the transitive paradigm, the ta- inverse marker never appears in the Intransitive-Active paradigm. This use of the itá auxiliary demonstrates an underlying split intransitive morphosyntactic alignment, specifically of the Split-S subtype: only verbs that indicate agency can license the itá auxiliary, whilst non-volitional verbs are restricted to the iná auxiliary.

Tense Number Indicative Inchoative Causative
 Present SG itá' yistá' sip'itá' (pis'itá')
PL itán yisnitán sipnitán (pisnitán)
Hodiernal Past SG hô'itá' yôsitá' hôsipnitá'
PL hônitán yôsintán hôsipnintán
Past SG nitá' yisnitá' sipnitá'
PL nintán yisnitán sipnintán
Medio-Distal Past SG hitá' yistá' hispitá'
PL hintán yisintán hisipnitán
Distal Past SG pitá' yipsitá' pisipta
PL pintán yipsintán pisipnitán
Medio-Proximal Future SG nitáh yisnitáh nisiptáh
PL nintahán yisnitahán nisipnitahán
Immediate Future SG hitáh yistáh hisiptahán
PL hintáhan yisnitahán hisipnitahán
Distal Future SG pitáh yipsitáh pisiptáh
PL pintáhan yipsiptahán pisipnitahán
Intransitive-Stative Paradigm

Intransitive verb structures involve pairing a semantically stative or non-agentive verb with the auiliary iná', a root cognate with the Minhast intransitive verbal ending -an. The intransitive paradigm of the auxiliary is listed in the table below:

Tense Number Indicative Inchoative Causative
 Present SG iná' inán sip'iná' (pis'iná')
PL niná' yisniná' sipniná' (pisniná')
Hodiernal Past SG hô'iná' yôsiná' hôsipniná'
PL hônniná' yôsinniná' hôsipnininá'
Past SG niná' yisniná' sipniná'
PL ninnininá' yisninniná' sipninniná'
Medio-Distal Past SG hiná' yisná' hispiná'
PL hinniná' yisninniná' hisipninniná'
Distal Past SG piná' yipsiná' pisipná'
PL pinniná' yipsininniná' pisipninniná'
Medio-Proximal Future SG nináh yisnináh nisipnáh
PL nininnináh yisninnninahá nisipninnináh
Immediate Future SG hináh yisnáh hisipináh
PL hinnináh yisninnináh hispinnináh
Distal Future SG pináh yipsináh pisipnáh
PL pinninnináh yipsipninnináh pisipninnináh
Nominalization

Nankôre has a rich set of clitics and suffixes that serve as nominalizers. They are, in order of frequency:

  • -pe: Clausal relativizer, cognate with Minhast =naft
Nan issiah pinninnináhpe pirút taporé ta'itá'.
nan issiah pinninnináh-pe pirút ta-poré ta-'itá'
man come COP.FUT-NMLZ sasquatch INV-vanquish INV-COP

The (next) man who comes here shall defeat the Sasquatch.
  • -hi[3][4]: attaches directly to a verb root to derive participles
  • -hici: Derives instruments from verb roots, a compound of -hi- and a submorpheme -ci, possibly derived from kaci "path"
  • -rē[5] : Derives occupation
  • -ssiak [6] Derives temporal adverbs from verbs, e.g. petissiak "time of/for standing", as well as creating adverbial clauses when attached to itá or iná , e.g.
Ak saraka nitássiak, sissioyenki kai pohráp niná'
ak saraka n-itá-ssiak, si=sioyenki kai pohráp n-iná'
1S see PST-AUX.TRN CONN=heart down fall PST-INTR

When I saw him, I became crestfallen.

The adverbial clause and its matrix may be inverted, e.g.:

Sioyenki kai pohráp ninássiak', siak saraka nitá.
sioyenki kai pohráp n-iná'-ssiak' si=ak saraka n-itá.
heart down fall PST-INTR CONN=1S see PST-AUX.TRN CONN=

I became crestfallen, having seen him.

The Connective si= obligatorily surfaces to link the adverbial clause with its matrix, regardless of the position of the two clauses.

Derivation

Nankôre has a set of verbalizing prefixes that when attached to nominal roots, often body parts, create verbs. These prefixes are most likely the remnants of Proto-Nahenic noun incorporation, based on comparisons with Nankôre's distant relatives, Minhast and Nahónda; similar or even identical fossilized affixes have been found in these languages that likewise attach to nominal roots for body parts to derive verbs.[7]


Verbalizing Prefixes
Prefix Sample Noun Example Meaning Nahenic Cognates
Sound k- ore "mouth" k-ore to speak Common Minhast k-irim "to speak" (NB: Nankôre ôre = "mouth" is cognate with Minhast erum = "mouth")
Striking ya- sipa "hand" ya-spa to punch Minhast, Horse Speaker dialect: wi-šnu "to strike, hit" (Minhast išna "fist", "knuckles")
Movement ho-
ha-
nake "foot" ha-nake to scoot along the floor with one's foot; to kick around Nahónda: ho-shishpa "to give" shishp = hand; c.f. Minhast sespir = hand, Nankôre sipa = hand
Stability pa- are "eye" p-are to stare Common Minhast p-uħta "to stand up" (from yuħta = "sole of the foot", cognate with Nankôre yohíhita = "flat surface")
Removal ro- ampe "body" ro-yampe to remove one's clothes Common Minhast ruyyamb "to take off clothing" (but note that Minhast lost the original *yambet (body), replacing it with tarti (body)

Numbers

The Nankôre number system is dozenal, i.e. base-12. The numbers thirteen through twenty-three are modified compounds consisting of the unit numbers siori (1) through siktas (11) compounded to sin (12). The number twenty-four hanosin can be analyzed as han-o-sin, where the affix -o- signifies "multiplied by", so hanosin literally means "two times twelve". The multiplicative pattern continues until the number 144, which is called nakpa. The number 100 (nenanosin >> nena ān-o-sin) is literally "four (and) eight times twelve". The ordinals for the numbers 1-7 are the cardinal numbers suffixed by -ak or -ok. Numbers 8-11 are the cardinal numbers suffixed with the ending -u. The ordinal for the number 12 is again suffixed with -ok, and the higher numbers with -nok. If the -nok affix is preceded by a consonant followed by -i-, the -i- may be dropped, provided that this does not form the impermissible consonant cluster -CCC- sequence.

Number Cardinal Ordinal
one siôri sioriak
two hani, ôhi hanyak
three tusta, tak tustak
four nena, kayoka nenak
five sisiak catak
six sirisio siirsiok
seven kerisio kirsiok
eight ān anku
nine karu karku
ten kase /'kaʃɛ/ kasku
eleven siktas sistu
twelve sin sinok
thirteen siorsin siorsinok
fourteen hansin hanisnok, hanisnok
fifteen tusin tusnok
sixteen ninsin ninsinok, nisnok
seventeen casin casinok,casnok
eighteen sirsin sirsinok, sirisnok
nineteen kirsin kirisnok
twenty ansin asnok
twenty-one karusin karushnok
twenty-two kasmin kasminok
twenty-three siksin siksinok
twenty-four hanosin hanosnok
thirty-six tustosin tustosnok
one hundred nenanosin nenanosnok
one hundred forty-four nakpa nakpanok

Clitics and Particles

Connective si=

Throughout all the Nahenic languages, connectives are used to bind dependents to their heads. Minhast has min, Neina the cognate mɯn, and Nahónda na, to join two or more noun phrases into one cohesive unit. Minhast has one additional connective, wa=, which joins non-verbal adjuncts, which may be a single word or an entire NP and/or adverbial phrase.

Nankóre's connective si= has no known cognates, and unlike its sister languages, si= is not restricted to joining NPs or complex, non-verbal phrases. While the dependency relation in a si= construction may be discernible, often the directionality is ambiguous and thus context must be used to disambiguate, or other morphosyntactic devices, such as the co-occurrence of the inverse marker ta' in possessive phrases.


Other Particles

Particle Meaning Cognates
ea
ira
yes Upper Minhast ēlā
ecá'
icá'
no Common Minhast hatā'
na and
so, therefore
Nahónda máma "also", Common Minhast =mā "and then"
piya but Neina (hi)yo
kusur then Neina kisero "afterwards"
ritá'a however Neina riha "to turn", Nahónda letsaya "to twist", Upper Minhast ruttakšuran "to seduce" (from Classical Minhast ruttay-gurra-ša'ra-ana "to swing one's hips")


Syntax

Word Order

Nankôre's canonical word order SOV, more specifically, SOV1V2, where V2 represents the auxiliary itá. Although the core argument NPs do not take any overt case marking, the animacy hierarchy and direct-inverse system of marking usually provide enough information to identify the Agent from the Patient. Thus, OSV1V2, SVO1 V2, and other orders are found, both in speech and text. When pronouns appear as core arguments, they usually appear before the main verb, but they may cliticize to the end of the main verb:

Minhaste kôryak itá'
/'minɦaʃte 'ko:rjak ɪt'aʔ/
minhast kôre=ak ∅-∅-itá-ʔ
Minhast.language speak=1S DIR-HS-COP-LS

I speak the Minhast language.

Nevertheless, for all word order variants, the only restriction is the auxiliary is restricted to clause-final position.

Fronting a constituent is used for emphasis, as in the following sample, where the O argument has been fronted before the S argument. Fronted constituents are typically followed by an audible pause, as in the following example:

Makse, rihat kakno itá
/'makʃɛ̯ ɾi'hat 'kakno ɪt'aʔ/
makse rihat kak=no ∅-∅-itá-ʔ
mouse.LA falcon.HA seize=SEM DIR-HS-COP-LS

It was the mouse the falcon seized in one fell swoop.

As in many SOV languages, modifiers precede their heads:

Sirkos sirohpa
/'ʃirkos ʃiroɦpa/
sirkos si=rohpa
foreigner fine.cloth

This fine foreign cloth

The Noun Phrase

Possession

Possession is indicated by joining the possessor to the possessum with the connective si=, followed either by the verb ras to indicate inalienable possession e.g. Koykare si=naho ras, "The boy's mother", or ocité for alienable possession, e.g. Koykare si=maska ocité. If the possessor is lower in the animacy heirarcy, the inverse marker ta'/tah is prefixed to the verb, as in the improbable Maska si=koykare tah-ocité, "The anthill's boy".

The Verb Phrase

The verb phrase is made up of the verb complex, and its predicate. The verb complex must end with the appropriate form of the auxiliary itá. Itá was originally a auxiliary but later on accreted voice, tense, and the pleonastic ta- Inverse marker. The main verb precedes the auxiliary and may be separated from it by a de-focused noun or by one or more adjuncts. De-focused nouns appear immediately after the main verb, and adjuncts cannot be inserted between the two (see "Quasi-Incorporation" below). Coverbs appear before the main verb. The primary ta- Inverse marker cliticizes to the first verb of the verb complex, which may be either a coverb, or the main verb.

Quasi-Noun Incorporation

Nankôre canonical word order is SOV1V2, but under certain circumstances, the O-argument may occur between V1 and V2, i.e. SV1OV2. The O-argument becomes backgrounded, thereby assuming a peripheral role; as a result, the clause is effectively detransitivized. As a result, the Inverse marker ta- cannot occur in these derived intransitive clauses; it must be omitted for the clause to remain grammatical. This operation is called quasi-noun incorporation, and occurs cross-linguistically in unrelated languages, e.g. Dutch and Hungarian. A feature of Nankôre psuedo-incorporation is that adjuncts, which typically can occur in most positions of a clause, including clause-finally, cannot occur between V1 and the relocated O-argument.

Quasi-incorporation may eventually evolve into full noun incorporation, as in the case of Nankôre's distant relatives, Minhast and Nahónda. Both exhibit full noun incorporation. Like Minhast and Nahónda, Nankôre adds the incorporated noun after the verb root, e.g. Minhast Ušnirumpakekaru << ušn-ruppumak-ek-ar-u (hit-face-3S.ABS+1S.ERG-PST.PFCT-TRANS) "I hit him in the face, I face-hit him", Nahónda klomenatsoyetolayo<< kloma-natsoye-Ø-t-ola-yo (speak-wisdom-3S.PT-1S.AGT-PST-AGT) "I gave him counsel, I wisdom-speak to him. Most languages that exhibit noun incorporation attach the incorporated noun before the verb root; placement of the incorporated noun after the root is rare among incorporating languages. The process by which Nankôre performs pseudo-incorporation, namely by placing the O-argument between the primary verb and auxiliary verbs, may explain why Minhast and Nahónda place their incorporated nouns after the verb. Proto-Nahenic may have also placed a quasi-incorporated noun after the primary verb but before the auxiliary verb, an order which the descendant languages preserved.

Quasi-Noun Incorporation serves discourse functions similar to full noun incorporation found in its Minhast and Nahónda relatives, backgrounding the incorporated NP, decreasing valency, and providing additional specificity to the semantics of the main verb. The following example of compares and contrasts the default SOV1V2 word order, with that of a SV1OV2:

  • Default SOV1V2 Order
Makse rihat tayôreno ta'itá
/'makʃɛ̯ ɾi'hat ta'yo:reno taʔɪt'aʔ/
makse rihat ta=yôre=no ta-itá-ʔ
mouse.LA falcon.HA INV=bite=SEM INV-HS.COP-LS

The mouse bit the falcon.
  • Quasi-Noun Incorporation, SV1OV2 Order
Makse yôreno rihat 'itá'
/'makʃɛ̯ 'yo:reno ɾi'hat taʔɪt'aʔ/
makse yôre=no rihat ∅-itá-ʔ
mouse.LA bite=SEM falcon DIR-COP-LS

The mouse falcon-bit.

Quasi-noun incorporation, like the full noun incorporation found in Nankôre's relatives, Minhast and Nahónda, lowers valency, transforming transitive clauses to intransitive clauses. The use of the Inverse marker is no longer needed, as the incorporated noun has been reduced to an adjunct. As such, the incorporated noun no longer occupies a core position. Decreasing the clause's valency reduces the saliency of the incorporated noun.

Comparatives and Superlatives

Degrees of comparison are formed by placing the coverb soyka (to be big, i.e. to be more) or hatka (to be small, i.e. to be less) before a stative verb. Since two arguments are involved, namely the comparer NP and the compared NP, the phrase is structurally equivalent to a transitive clause, requiring an Agent and a Patient, and follows the rules governing the nominal animacy hierarchy. In the sentence Joe Sara soyka inupe itá (Joe is stronger than Sarah; lit. "Joe bigs strongs Sara"), soyka precedes the stative verb inupe (to be strong). Joe, being male, is higher in the animacy scale and so is assigned Agent status. If Sara were stronger, the Inverse affix ta-/tā-/tāh- surfaces, as in Joe Sara ta-soyka inupe ta'itá (Sara is stronger than Joe). Other examples:

1) Joe Sara soyka soyka ta'itá (Joe is taller than Sara; lit. "Joe big bigs Sara").

2) Joe Sara tā-hatka nahamos ta'itá (Sara is less tan than Joe).

3) Joe Sara hatka hatka ta'itá (Joe is smaller than Sara; lit. "Joe small smalls Sara").

4) Natos Suhe soyka makit itá (Natosh [masc.] runs faster than Suhe [fem.]; lit. "Natosh big runs Suhe").

5) Orôyo Paul ta-soyka soyka itá (The volcano is bigger than Paul; note that orôyo is less animate because it is a sessile object, hence the use of the inverse marker ta-).

To express the superlative, the auxiliary verb norhe/norhâ appears before soyka and hatka, e.g. Orôyo norhe soyka soyka itá (This is the largest mountain), or Sikôya orôyo norhe soyka soyka itá (This is the largest mountain in Kôya Island).

Example texts

The Tower of Babel

  1. Now the whole earth had one language and the same words.
  2. And as people migrated from the east, they found a plain in the land of Shinar and settled there.
  3. And they said to one another, "Come, let us make bricks, and burn them thoroughly." And they had brick for stone, and bitumen for mortar.
  4. Then they said, "Come, let us build ourselves a city and a tower with its top in the heavens, and let us make a name for ourselves, lest we be dispersed over the face of the whole earth."
  5. And the Lord came down to see the city and the tower, which the children of man had built.
  6. And the Lord said, "Behold, they are one people, and they have all one language, and this is only the beginning of what they will do. And nothing that they propose to do will now be impossible for them.
  7. Come, let us go down and there confuse their language, so that they may not understand one another's speech."
  8. So the Lord dispersed them from there over the face of all the earth, and they left off building the city.
  9. Therefore its name was called Babel, because there the Lord confused the language of all the earth. And from there the Lord dispersed them over the face of all the earth.


  1. Saska nammanuyyi nan siôri si-kôre maskoy pintá', kor-kôre nihe pinninnináh.
    Now in.those.days mankind one CONN-speech have AUX.TRN+DIST.PST+HP.LS, word-word resemble AUX.INTR+DIST.PST.PL
  2. Iape nanni hóttari pinninnináh, coy siori pintá', Sinar rima pintá', itáp pinninnináh.
    So horde approach.setting.sun AUX.INTR+DIST.PST.PL, forest.clearing see AUX.TRN+DIST.PST+HP.LS, Sinar call AUX.TRN+DIST.PST+HP.LS, sit.down AUX.INTR+DIST.PST.PL

Table of Abbreviations

Abbreviation Meaning
C Single Consonant
V Single Vowel
VV Long/Geminate Vowel
PN Proper Noun
NP Noun Phrase
VP Verb Phrase
OBL Oblique argument
DEM Demonym
AGT Agent
PT Patient
DIR Direct Voice
INV Inverse Voice
CONN Connective
HA High Animate
HS High Animate Singular
HP High Animate Plural
LA Low Animate
LS Low Animate Singular
LP Low Animate Plural
NEG Negator
CAUS Causative
INCH Inchoative
SEM Semelfective
PNI Pseudo-Noun Incorporation
PIN Pseudo-Incorporated Noun
SG Singular
PL Plural
REM.PST Remote Past Tense
HOD Hodiurnal Past Tense
PST Past Tense
PRS Present Tense
IMM.FUT Immediate Future Tense
FUT Future Tense
IMPF Imperfect Aspect
PRF Perfect Aspect
IRR Irrealis

Other resources

  1. ^ Found mostly in older linguistic texts before 1965. The character has largely fell into disuse afterwards.
  2. ^ From reconstructed P-Nahenic *wa=man=kor (CONN=SEQ=and.yet).
  3. ^ Not to be confused for Irrealis -hi, which suffixes to the itá'/iná' auxiliaries.
  4. ^ Cognate with Minhast gerundial -x-
  5. ^ Cognate with Minhast redad "man/male/person
  6. ^ Derived from siriak, "appointed time". Cognate with Minhast saħrap "today"
  7. ^ C.f. Minhast kirim (from k-erum, literally "make sound with the mouth) and Nahónda teloma (from t-loma). Both are cognates of Nankôre's kôre "speech".