Nankôre: Difference between revisions

From Linguifex
Jump to navigation Jump to search
Line 480: Line 480:
|-  
|-  
! Riverine  
! Riverine  
| temen|| Kurasno temen rohnas-kor itá.|| The girls supposedly were playing by the river.||
| temen|| Kurasno temen-kor rohnas itá.|| The girls supposedly were playing by the river.||
|-  
|-  
! Montaigne  
! Montaigne  

Revision as of 00:02, 3 August 2014


Nankôre
Nankôre
Pronunciation[/nan.'ko:re/]
Created by
Native toNanhoshka Kôya
Native speakers3,232,430 (2014)
Language Isolate
  • Nankôre
Official status
Official language in
Koya Island
Language codes
ISO 639-1nk
ISO 639-2nnk

Background

Nankôre, from the words nan ("man, human") and kôre ("speech"), is spoken by the Nanhoshka people of Kôya Island. There are two main dialects, the Konishmak, located in the Northeastern Coastal Mountains, and the Sapshira, encompassing the western and southern parts of the country. The dialects differ chiefly in pronunciation and differences in certain lexical items, but are otherwise mutually intelligible.

The origins of the language and its people remain clouded in mystery. Physically, the Nanhoshka people look distinct from the Northwest Pacific Native Americans just 862 kilometers from the eastern shore, but their physical features bear a strikingly close resemblance to the Minhast people of the Republic of Minhay, who live much further away on the other side of the Pacific Ocean close to Japan and Ainushir. Because of these physical resemblences, both anthropologists and linguists have been exploring a possible relationship between the two peoples. Although recent genetic research has uncovered that both the Nanhoshka and Minhast belong to the haplogroup C-M212, suggesting a common ancestry, linguists have been much less successful in establishing a relationship. Some linguists have compiled Swadesh lists that suggest some lexical items may have a common origin, but so far researchers have failed to demonstrate reliable sound correspondences between the two languages. Therefore, both Nankôre and Minhast remain classified as language isolates.




Phonology

Consonants

Nankôre has a small inventory of consonants, only thirteen in number. Noteworthy is the paucity of voiced consonants which contributes to the limited number of consonants in the Nankôre language. The following table contains the entire consonantal inventory:

Bilabial Dental Alveolar Post-alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
Nasal m n
Stop p t k ʔ
Affricate ͡tʃ
Fricative s ʃ h
Approximant w j
Flap r

Vowels

  Front Near- front Central Near- back Back
Close
Blank vowel trapezoid.svg
i
u
ɪ
e
o
ɛ
a
  Near-close
Close-mid
Mid
Open-mid
Near-open
Open

Phonotactics

Orthography

The Nankôre orthography is based on the Allen-Mills system, derived from the Latin alphabet:

Grapheme IPA
' ʔ
a ɑ
e e
i ɪ, i
o o
u u
c
s s, ʃ
sh ʃ
h h
k k
r r
m m
n n
p p
t t
w w


Note that the grapeme <s> is pronounced /ʃ/ when followed by [i] (e.g. sinkokah "orca"), or in coda position (Nanhoska "the Nanhoshka people"). A sound shift occurred in the Konismak dialect that spread to the southeastern portion of the country a few decades after the Allen-Mills system was devised. The digraph <sh> was retained for all other cases.

Grammar

Morphology

Nouns

The Nominal Heirarchy

The nominal system, at first glance, appears very simple. They are not inflected for case, gender, nor do adpositions indicate their directional or positional relationships. Number is distinguished only in pronouns. Nevertheless, the nominal system has a complex heirarchy of animacy, which although unmarked, is a semantic feature of the noun and pronoun. Where a given NP falls within the animacy heirarchy must be memorized in order to use the verbal inverse prefix tā(h)- correctly (see section on Verbs regarding inverse marking).

The Nankôre nominal heirarchy has been traditionally classified along two axes, the first marking category, and the second marking levels of heirarchy within a given category. Animacy along categories decrease as one traverses from Supernatural/Phenomonological onwards towards the Ephemeral. Animacy decreases as one goes down the Level axis. The heirarchy is arranged according to the following table:

Table of Nominal Animacy Heirarchy
Level Supernatural &
Phenomonological
Human Animals Weather Lower Animals Plants Geophysical Material Ephemeral
Mammals Birds
1 Gods Adults >> Children predator >> prey Sleet Reptiles (excluding snakes) Forests Rushing rivers Water Magic
2 Demons Older >> Younger wild >> domesticated Rain Snakes Large Trees Slow-moving rivers Food Emotions
3 Human & Animal Spirits Male >> Female Pet (usually carnivorous) >> Non-Pet (usually raised for food) Snow Amphibians Small Trees Streams, brooks, springs Fabrics, fibers, skins, clothing Actions
4 Destructive Forces of Nature Strong >> Weak Large >> Small Wind Fish Fruit/Nut Trees Lakes Wood Thoughts
5 Lightning Large >> Small Strong >> Weak Breeze Crustaceans Bushes/Shrubs Glaciers Buildings Disease
6 Ocean Quick >> Slow Quick >> Slow Water Insects Prairie grass Mountains Metals States
7 Thunderstorms Intelligent >> Unintelligent Carnivore >> Herbivore >> Carrion Eater Air Worms Leafy plants Cliffs, jagged outcroppings, tumuli, boulders Jewels and gemstones Abstractions
8 Fire Fertile >> Infertile Stud >> Gelded Flight >> Flightless Muggy weather Mollusks Mushrooms/Fungi/Mosses Flat land or gently rolling landscape Stones, rocks, pebbles Death
Pronouns

The first group of pronouns are the simple independent pronouns. The animacy of each pronoun is arranged in the following heirarchy:

Pronominal Heirarchy
1st >> 2nd >> 3rd

Plurality generally does not affect the animacy level across persons. However, if two 3rd person arguments have the same animacy and one of the arguments is plural, then the plural argument is assigned a higher animacy level.

The following table lists the forms of the simple independent pronouns:

Simple Independent Pronouns
Singular Plural
1S ak akôs
2S kakôs
3S takôs

If used as a core argument, they appear in intransitive clauses only, and just before the clause-final particle itá. They may also be used to emphasize a noun, in which case the pronoun precedes the noun linked with the connective si=, as in tā si=naho (She/Her, the mother).


Portmanteau Pronouns
Singular Plural
1S 2S 3S 1P 2P 3P
1S

--

akak akat

--

akakos akatos
2S akak

--

kat akoska

--

katakos
3S akat kat tat akosta kakosta takosta
Plural x Plural
1P

--

akoskos akostos
2P akoskos

--

kakostos
3P akostos kakostos takostos

As earlier stated, nouns in Nankôre are not inflected, nor are case clitics attached to them to indicate case relations. Transitive verbs take two NPs as core arguments. The case relations of the two arguments are determined by the position of the noun in the animacy heirarchy; the core argument that is higher in the animacy heirarchy is assigned the Agent role, and the other argument is assigned the Patient role. However, an inverse affix tā-/tāh- is prefixed to the verb or its auxiliary to alter the argument structure by marking the lower-animacy argument as Agent.

Oblique arguments on the other hand are treated as adjuncts, and are thus bound to their clause with the connective clitic si= or =si. Context alone determines the semantic role of the oblique. Because obliques are considered adjuncts, they may not come between the core arguments and the verb, and therefore must appear either before or after the clause nucleus. To illustrate, the sentence Maska=si ohipna koykare ekán itá (anthill=CONN twig boy twist PST), i.e. "The boy inserted the twig into the anthill", the noun maska (anthill) is the oblique argument and is marked with the clitic si= to join it to the rest of the sentence. Oblique arguments use the form =si preceding the clause nucleus, or si= following the clause nucleus, e.g. Ohipna koykare ekán itá si=maska.

Verbs

Verbs in Nankôre, as in many other languages, can perform valence operations to alter the argument structure of a clause. Agents and Patients may be demoted from their core status, Patients may be promoted to a derived Subject, and Obliques may be promoted to a derived Patient role. Nankôre has a several auxiliary verbs that perform passivation, antipassivation, and applicativization. In a language that has virtually no adpositions or case markings on the NPs, valence operations conducted by these auxiliary verbs, particularly the applicative auxiliaries, are exploited to encode spatial or directional information that case affixes or adpositions perform in other languages. The animacy system interacts with these auxiliary verbs, called valence auxiliaries in the linguistic literature of Nankôre.

As an example, the auxiliary kohán passivizes the clause, resulting in a one-argument clause containing only the logical Patient. Since there is only one core argument, the need for inverse marking no longer exists, and in fact the presence of both the passive auxiliary kohán and the inverse marker tā-/tāh-' in the same sentence is ungrammatical. The same is true for the antipassive auxilliary norhe/norhâ, that is the logical Patient is deleted leaving only the agent, and so once again, the presence of the inverse tā-/tāh- is ungrammatical. When either the Agent or the Patient has been deleted by the passive or antipassive auxiliaries, one thing that must be noted is that the demoted arguments are fully deleted. In other languages that have passives and/or antipassives, demoted core arguments need not be deleted, in fact in many languages the demoted argument may still be retained in the sentence, but this time as an oblique argument. Such is not the case in Nankôre; the former core argument cannot appear in the sentence.

The applicative auxiliaries promote an Oblique argument to core status, thereby creating a derived Patient. This results in a transitive sentence, and the inverse marker tā-/tāh- once again can be used to disambiguate the roles of the core arguments if a lower-animacy argument is an Agent. Interestingly, unlike passivation and antipassivation, the demoted core argument, which is always the former Patient, can still appear in the clause as an Oblique argument, marked with the connective si- There are several of these auxiliaries, listed in the following table:

Table of Applicative Auxiliary Verbs
Auxiliary Sample Sentence Translation Comment
Directional sanha Anut America sanha-kor kamuk itá. Anut supposedly flew to America. -kor = hearsay affix, attaches to the auxiliary.
Benefactive nasko Nanhoska karen tā-nasko-ro-kor eyna itá. It is said the (sacred) tree gave (life) for the people. -ro- = Imperfect aspect. Note also the inverse marker tā-
Commitative yampe Anut yampe temen itá Anut walked beside the river with him. temen = to perform an activity by a river
Instrumental makôr Ehok yurasna makôr-nahoyra-kor neat itá. The adolescent struck (it) repeatedly with a club. -nahoyra- = Repetitive
Ablative risa (Example) (Example)
Locative neyhi (Example) (Example)
Vialis nahke (Example) (Example)
Superessive oros (Example) (Example)
Subessive yorha (Example) (Example)
Ablative-Superessive isuk (Example) (Example)
Ablative-Subessive royna (Example) (Example)
Riverine temen Kurasno temen-kor rohnas itá. The girls supposedly were playing by the river.
Montaigne yayak (Example) (Example)

These auxiliaries are often confused for adpositions, or relational nouns, but unlike adpositions and locative nouns, these auxiliaries are full-fledged verbs. If inverse marking is required, the prefix tā/tāh is still prefixed to the auxiliary verb. Likewise, aspect and modality markers are attached to the auxiliary, a process that occurs only with verbs in the language, never with nouns. Moreover, these auxiliaries can stand alone as independent verbs, such as Kosmakis Anut yampe itá "Anut walked with Koshmak" (yampe = to be arm-in-arm; c.f. yanup "elbow").


Syntax

The Noun Phrase (NP)

Possession

Possession is indicated by joining the possessor to the possessum with the connective si=, followed either by the verb ras to indicate inalienable possession e.g. Koykare si=naho ras, "The boy's mother", or ocité for alienable possession, e.g. Koykare si=maska ocité. If the possessor is lower in the animacy heirarcy, the inverse marker ta'/tah is prefixed to the verb, as in the improbable Maska si=koykare tah-ocité, "The anthill's boy".

Comparatives and Superlatives

Degrees of comparison are formed by placing the auxiliary verb soyka (to be big, i.e. to be more) or hatka (to be small, i.e. to be less) before a stative verb. Since two arguments are involved, namely the comparer NP and the compared NP, the phrase is structurally equivalent to a transitive clause, requiring an Agent and a Patient, and follows the rules governing the nominal animacy heirarchy. In the sentence Joe Sara soyka inupe tā itá (Joe is stronger than Sarah; lit. "Joe bigs strongs Sara"), soyka precedes the stative verb inupe (to be strong). Joe, being male, is higher in the animacy scale and so is assigned Agent status. If Sara were stronger, the Inverse affix tā-/tāh- surfaces, as in Joe Sara -soyka inupe tā itá (Sara is stronger than Joe). Other examples:

1) Joe Sara soyka soyka tā itá (Joe is taller than Sara; lit. "Joe bigs bigs Sara").

2) Joe Sara tā-hatka nahamosh tā itá (Sara is less tan than Joe).

3) Joe Sara hatka hatka tā itá (Joe is smaller than Sara; lit. "Joe small smalls Sara").

4) Natos Suhe soyka makit itá (Natosh [masc.] runs faster than Suhe [fem.]; lit. "Natosh big runs Suhe").

5) Orôyo Paul tā-soyka soyka itá (The volcano is bigger than Paul; note that orôyo is less animate because it is a sessile object, hence the use of the inverse marker tā-).


To express the superlative, the clause must be antipassivized with the auxiliary verb norhe/norhâ e.g. Orôyo norhe soyka soyka itá (This is the largest mountain), or Sikôya orôyo norhe soyka soyka itá (This is the largest mountain in Kôya Island).