Proto-Antarctican
The Proto-Antarctican language evolved in the generations after the colonisation of the Antarctican continent. Later on, it evolved into the Antarctican language. It was influenced by a large variety of languages spoken in areas where the colonisers hailed from (Spanish and Portuguese from South America, English from Australia, and Japanese, Taiwanese, Tagalog, Vietnamese, Khmer, Thai, Burmese and Indonesian from East / SE Asia).
Phonology
Consonants
The consonant inventory is broadly similar to that of Antarctican. The most striking difference is the lack of lateral obstruents, and with a set of palatalised alveolar consonants instead (which indeed did become lateral consonants later). There are no velar and palatal nasals at all, however the pre-stopped nasals are in existence. There are also no ejectives.
Bilabial | Alveolar | Palatal | Velar | Glottal / Placeless | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
plain | palatalised | plain | palatalised | |||||
Nasals | plain | m | mʲ | n | nʲ | |||
pre-stopped | pm | pmʲ | tn | tnʲ |
|
|||
Stops | voiceless | p | pʲ | t | tʲ | c | k | ʔ |
voiced | b | bʲ | d | dʲ | ɟ | g | ||
Fricatives | voiceless | f | fʲ | s | sʲ | ç | x | |
voiced | z | zʲ | ||||||
Approximants | w | ɥ | ɫ | ʎ | j |
Vowels
Proto-Antarctican has a simpler inventory of vowels than Antarctican. It is worth noting here that, as yet, the language has no phonation distinctions on vowels.
Monophthongs
Front | Central | Back | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Short | Long | Short | Long | Short | Long | |
High | i | iː | u | uː | ||
Mid | (e) | (eː) | əː | o | oː | |
Low | a | aː |
The vowel /i/ can only be followed by palatal or palatalised consonants. The vowel /e/ can never be followed by these consonants. Thus it could be said that they are allophones.
Diphthongs
Proto-Antarctican also had 8 short diphthongs /ei ai oi ui iu eu au ou/ and 6 long diphthongs /eːi aːi oːi eːu aːu oːu/.
Nasal Vowels
All monophthongs and diphthongs in Proto-Antarctican had an phonemic contrast between oral vowels and nasal vowels. These are marked with a tilde e.g. /tẽc/ - weather.
Phonotactics
In contrast to the smaller amount of phonemes of Proto-Antarctican compared to Antarctican, Proto-Antarctican permitted a much larger array of syllable structures.
In general, the range of permitted syllable shapes was C V (O), where O is an obstruent (fricative or oral stop). However, the first syllable of a word could begin with up to two consonants i.e. the range of permitted syllable shapes here was (C) C V (O).
/t/ cannot come before /u/. If a process such as compounding would bring them together, an epenthetic /s/ is inserted (this is an influence from Japanese).
There were also restrictions on consonant clusters. Not only did they have to be homorganic for voicing (i.e. clusters like /tb/ and /zk/ were not permitted, but /tp/, /db/, /zg/ and /sk/ were), but they also had to be homorganic for palatalisation (i.e. clusters like /spʲ/ were not permitted, but /sʲpʲ/ and /sp/ were).
Also, consonant clusters could only consist of obstruents, not sonorants. So clusters like /nt/ were not permitted.
For the purposes of Proto-Antarctican phonology, pre-stopped nasals count as sonorants and not as obstruents. Hence they could not be part of consonant clusters or form the coda of the last syllable of a word.
Consonant Gemination
There was a phonemic gemination contrast in obstruents. This could occur regardless of whether they were voiced or voiceless. However, there were a few restrictions on where such geminated consonants could occur:
- They either had to occur between two vowels, or at the end of words. So /takː/ and /takːa/ were permitted, but not /tːak/, /takːta/ or /taktːa/.
- They could not occur after a nasal vowel or diphthong, but could occur before them. So /takːã/ and /takːai/ ware permitted, but not /tãkːa/ or /taikːa/.
- They could never occur after a long vowel. So words like /taːkːa/ were not permitted.
- It was quite rare for them to occur before a long vowel either, but not impossible.
- It was rare for a word to have more than one geminated consonant (in contrast to long vowels, where there were many words which contained multiples of them).
Stress
Stress in Proto-Antarctican was predictable, falling on the first heavy syllable of a word (one followed by a geminated consonant or a pre-stopped nasal, or one containing a long vowel, a diphthong, a nasal vowel or a coda consonant). If a word contained no heavy syllables, then stress would fall on the second syllable.
Grammar
Nouns
Proto-Antarctican originally borrowed its noun morphology open slather from Japanese. It used postpositions to mark nominative case /ga/, accusative case /wo/ (a spelling pronunciation), topicalisation /wa/, comitative case /mo/, genitive case /no/, and dative case /nʲi/ etc. It did have one extra case, an alienable possessive case marked with the postposition /wei/ (/no/ was used exclusively for inalienable possession).
The language also had pronominal possessive particles, borrowed from English and Spanish, though these were marked for clusivity due to influence from Austronesian languages:
- /mi/ 1st person exclusive
- /jau/ 1st person inclusive
- /te/ 2nd person
- /su/ 3rd person
However, major changes occurred to the case system. Firstly, /no/ and /wei/ stopped being interpreted as postpositions attaching to the possessor noun, and instead became prepositions attaching to the possessed noun, occupying the same syntactic "slot" as pronominal possessive prefixes.
In addition, the topic particle /wa/ was lost everywhere. Henceforth, topicalisation was indicated solely by word order. The only exception was where it was reinterpreted as a possessive particle indicating that the noun was possessed by the topic. The particle /su/ was interpreted as indicating the noun was possessed by something other than the topic.
The accusative particle /wo/ was also lost, except when it was needed to distinguish direct from indirect objects.
Now, Proto-Antarctican was still a nominative-accusative language, but one that only marked the nominative. In intransitive sentences, this was redundant, and the postposition /ga/ was dropped. Now it only marked the subject of an intransitive sentence, and was hence an ergative case marker.
At this point, the language now used prepositions more than postpositions, which triggered a massive change in the syntax to become head-initial.
Eventually, the prepositions fused to the noun as prefixes. The ergative postposition /ga/ fused to the noun as a suffix. If the noun ended in a vowel, an epenthetic /u/ was inserted e.g. /bukː/ - book, absolutive -> /bukkuga/ - book, ergative. If the noun ended in a palatalised consonant, an epenthetic /i/ was inserted instead e.g. /tẽc/ - weather, absolutive -> /tẽciga/. And if the noun ended in /t/ or /d/, /o/ was inserted instead e.g. /ʔoiʎãd/ - island, absolutive -> /ʔoiʎãdoga/ - island, ergative.
Verbs
Pronominal Objects
These were originally marked by prefixes (a feature borrowed from Spanish). However, early in the development of Proto-Antarctican, they underwent metathesis to become infixes (which came immediately before the first vowel). There were two separate forms of the infix, one for words beginning with a palatalised consonant, and another for words beginning with a non-palatalised consonant. The consonants inside the infix must agree with the initial consonant in palatalisation:
- 1st Person Exclusive: /imʲ/ ~ /em/ (from Spanish "mi")
- 1st Person Inclusive: /asʲ/ ~ /as/ (from English "as")
- 2nd Person: /əːsʲ/ ~ /əːs/ (from Spanish "vos")
- Reflexive: /iʎ/ ~ /eɫ/ (from Spanish "él" which underwent semantic shift)
- 3rd Person Singular: /utʲ/ ~ /ut/ (from English "it", extended to cover both animate and inanimate objects).
- 3rd Person Plural: /amʲ/ ~ /am/ (from English "them").
However, with the development of ergativity, the 3rd person pronominal infixes shifted in meaning to mark antipassive voice. The plural pronoun came to mean that the action happened multiple times, eventually turning into a portmanteau morpheme marking both antipassive voice and imperfective aspect. Similarly, the singular pronoun came to mark both antipassive voice and perfective aspect.
Pronominal Subjects
Subject pronouns also fused as affixes on the verb. However, since most of them were borrowed from English, which fused tense marking onto pronouns via contractions with an auxiliary verb (e.g. marking both 1st person singular subject and future tense by "I'll"), they also marked tense.
Past | Present | Future | |
---|---|---|---|
1PS Exclusive | ʔoi | ʔomʲi | ʔou |
1PS Inclusive | ci | we | wu |
2PS | nʲĩ | jo | ju |
3PS | zai | ze | zau |
Who / What | wo | heu | ɫu |
Apart from this, tense and aspect were not marked on Proto-Antarctican verbs.
Verbalisation
To change a noun into a verb, the typical suffix was /z/ e.g.
/sʲəː/ - show
/sʲəːz/ - to perform
If the verb root already ended in a consonant, one of two things happened:
- If the consonant was a non-geminated voiced obstruent, the suffix was changed to /zu/ e.g. /doʎaːg/ - something being dragged -> /doʎaːgzu/ - to drag
- Otherwise, the suffix was changed to /iz/ after a palatalised consonant, and /uz/ otherwise e.g. /damas/ - a scam -> /damasuz/ - to scam or /sakːʲ/ - past event -> /sakːʲiz/ - to have already occurred some time ago.
Any noun could be verbalised to a verb meaning "to be ..." or "to become ..." by simply applying either this transformation e.g.
/zuɥa/ - boat
/zuɥaz/ - to be a boat
Adverbs
Nouns could also be changed into adverbs. This was originally done by suffixing them with /i/. However this underwent metathesis with the final vowel, and ended up as simple palatalisation of the final consonant if it was non-palatalised, and as an infix /iʔ/ if the last consonant of the word was already palatalised e.g.
/jĩtoɫəː/ - introduction
/jĩtoʎəː/ - as an introduction
/ʔumupo/ - seat
/ʔumupʲo/ - sitting down (used as an adverb)
/sʲəː/ - show
/sʲiʔəː/ - showing off (used as an adverb)
Adaptations of Words from Present-Day Languages
Proto-Antarctican vocabulary is descended from a variety of present day languages. However, many words have undergone sound changes to fit Proto-Antarctican phonology.
English
A very large proportion of Proto-Antarctican vocabulary can be traced back to present day English (mainly Australian English, with some influence from New Zealand and South African English). However, they are sometimes not recognisable as such because, apart from other things, many consonant clusters have been broken up by inserting /u/ (/i/ if after a palatalised consonant, /o/ if after /t/ or /d/).
Vowels
- The diphthong in English "FEAR" has monophthongised, becoming a long vowel /iː/.
- The monophthong in English "SEA" has diphthongised, becoming /ei/.
- The diphthong in "SAY" corresponds to Proto-Antarctican /ai/.
- The diphthong in "SIGH" corresponds to Proto-Antarctican /oi/.
- The diphthong is "SOY" corresponds to Proto-Antarctican /ui/.
- English "dark l" has vocalised, creating new diphthongs ending in /u/ e.g. /iu/, /eu/, /au/, /ou/. The sequence in English "CALL" became a long diphthong /oːu/, while the sequence in English "COAL" became a short diphthong /ou/. After a diphthong, dark l became a separate syllable /ju/ e.g. English "MILE" became /moju/.
- The vowel in "FAIR" corresponds to Proto-Antarctican /eː/.
- The vowel in "ADD" corresponds to Proto-Antarctican /ja/ if word initial, and /ʲa/ with palatalisation on the preceding consonant if not word initial e.g. in "LAD".
- Since the major dialect of English which influenced Proto-Antarctican was Australian English (which has undergone the bad-lad split), the vowel in "ANNE" corresponds to Proto-Antarctican /jaː/ if word initial, and /ʲaː/ if not e.g. in the word "BAD".
- The vowels in English "BARN" and "BURN" have merged to /aː/.
- Short schwa has lowered to /a/.
- The diphthong in "KNOW" has monophthongised to a long schwa /əː/.
- The vowel in "COT" has become /o/.
- The vowel in "COURT" or "CAUGHT" has become /oː/.
- In some words, the vowel in English "TWO" became a diphthong /eu/ (and thus merging with "TELL"). This sound change did not happen consistently across the language. In some cases, the diphthong was lengthened, in other cases it was not, and in some cases the sound change did not happen at all, leaving the vowel as /uː/. Note that it was always blocked after /j/ or before liquid consonants.
- The diphthong in English "SEWER" was usually split into two syllables, becoming /uɥa/. However, this was not consistent across the language, sometimes becoming /uwa/, /uː/ or /oː/.
Consonants
- English clusters of fricative + nasal have become prestopped nasals, at the point of articulation of the nasal e.g. English "smart" -> P.A. /pmaːt/.
- English nasals at the end of syllables have usually disappeared, nasalising the preceding vowel. Sometimes /m/ has survived by acquiring an epenthetic /u/ after it, becoming /mu/. And sometimes /ŋ/ became /gu/ with nasalisation on the preceding vowel.
- English dental fricatives merged with their alveolar counterparts.
- English light l became /ɫ/ (the same pronunciation as present day English dark l, which vocalised in Proto-Antarctican). The only exception to this was before /i/ or /l/, when it became /ʎ/ instead.
- English /ɹ/ usually merged with light l, and followed the same process as above. However, sometimes it merged with /w/ instead.
- English postalveolar consonants have become palatalised alveolar in Proto-Antarctican.
- English /v/ merged into /b/ (or sometimes /w/), except for the cluster /vj/ (e.g. in "VIEW") which became /ɥ/.
- English /w/ (and sometimes /v/) became /ɥ/ before the "LAD" and "BAD" vowels e.g. English "wag" -> P.A. /ɥaːg/, English "vagabond" -> P.A. /ɥagːabõd/.
- English /h/ became /x/, except before /i/ or in the cluster /hj/ (e.g. in "HUMAN") which became /ç/.
- If an English stressed short monophthong was immediately followed by an obstruent (with no consonants intervening), and then either a word boundary or another vowel (which could be epenthetic) after that, then that obstruent tended to become geminated e.g. English "book" -> P.A. /bukː/, English "office" -> P.A. /ʔofːʲis/. However this sound change was not consistent across the language e.g. English "brother" -> P.A. /buɫaza/, not /buɫazːa/.
- English /t/ and /dz/ became /ts/ and /z/ before /u/.
Japanese
Quite a lot of Proto-Antarctican vocabulary also comes from Japanese. In general, these words did not need to undergo many sound changes to be compatible with Proto-Antarctican phonology, since Japanese phonology is very restrictive. However, there were a few that need mentioning:
- Japanese short /i/ and /u/ (but not /ju/) were deleted between non-geminated obstruents with similar voicing e.g. Japanese /youfuku/ - clothes -> P.A. /joːfku/., Japanese /zubon/ - pants -> P.A. /zbõ/
- Japanese short /i/ or /u/ (but not /ju/) were deleted at the end of a word, as long as they were not preceded by a consonant cluster e.g. Japanese /sakki/ - some time ago -> P.A. /sakːʲ/, Japanese /mazu/ - firstly -> P.A. /maz/.
- Japanese /ɯ/ became /u/.
- Japanese /tts/ became /tːos/, or /tos/ after a long vowel or diphthong.
- Japanese /r/ became /ɫ/, except before /i/ when it became /ʎ/.
- Japanese palatalised /rʲ/ became /ʎ/.
- Japanese /au/ became /əː/.
- Japanese /ao/ became /au/ (i.e. took part in a chain shift with /au/.
- Japanese /ae/ merged into /ai/.
- Japanese word final /iru/, /eru/, /oru/ and /uru/ became /iu/, /eu/, /ou/ and /uː/ respectively e.g. Japanese /kanjiru/ - to feel -> P.A. /kãdʲiu/, Japanese /nagareru/ - to flow -> P.A. /nagaɫeu/, Japanese /shiboru/ - to squeeze -> P.A. /sʲibou/, Japanese /tsuru/ - to hang -> P.A. /tsuː/.
- If the preceding sound change would create a tripthong, it was broken up into two syllables by inserting a semivowel (/w/ if the first vowel was back, and /j/ otherwise) e.g. Japanese /kazoeru/ -> to count -> P.A. /kazoweu/, Japanese /naoru/ - to get better -> P.A. /najou/.
Spanish
Owing to the proximity of South America, Proto-Antarctican was heavily influenced by Latin American Spanish (which has seseo and yeísmo).
- Spanish ñ became /nʲ/ e.g. Spanish mañana - morning -> P.A. /manʲana/.
- Obstruents immediately after a stressed vowel were geminated where possible e.g. Spanish cabeza - head -> P.A. /kabesːa/.
- If Spanish r is not before a vowel, it is deleted with lengthening of the preceding vowel e.g. Spanish pensar - to think -> P.A. /pẽsaː/ - to be lost in deep thought.
- If Spanish r is word initial, or doubled, then it changed to /z/ e.g. Spanish tierra - land -> P.A. /teza/ - landscape.
- Otherwise, Spanish r merged into l.
- Before /i/, Spanish l (and single r) was changed to /ʎ/ e.g. Spanish Lima - Lima (in Peru) -> P.A. /ʎima/.
- Before other vowels, Spanish l (and single r) was changed to /ɫ/ e.g. Spanish palo - stick -> P.A. /paɫo/. Spanish derecha - right (as in left/right) -> P.A. /deɫetːʲa/.
- If not before a vowel, Spanish l was vocalised. After /u/ it disappeared entirely, and after other vowels it created diphthongs ending in /u/ e.g. Spanish golpear - to hit -> P.A. /goupejaː/ - to suffer a setback.
- Spanish /ʝ/ and non-syllabic /i/ both merged into /j/ before a vowel, and /i/ otherwise.
- Spanish non-syllabic /u/ became /w/ before a vowel e.g. Spanish huevos - eggs -> P.A. /webːos/. However, it coalesced with /x/ to become /f/ e.g. Spanish juego - game -> P.A. /fegːo/. If it came after any other consonant, an epenthetic /u/ was inserted e.g. Spanish fuego - fire -> P.A. /fuwegːo/
- Spanish /t/ and /d/ becomes /ts/ and /z/ before /u/.
- Spanish /i/ always palatalised preceding consonants e.g. Spanish viuda - widow -> P.A. /bʲudːa/.
- Spanish /e/ always depalatalised preceding consonants e.g. Spanish noche - night -> P.A. /notːe/.
- Spanish ea and eo became /eja/ and /ejo/ respectively.
- If not before a consonant, Spanish nasals were deleted with nasalisation on the preceding vowel.
Burmese
Another language which had heavy influence on Proto-Antarctican was Burmese. Below are the sound changes of note that happened to Burmese loanwords in the language:
- Vowels and diphthongs with low (modal) tone became lengthened word finally, and are short otherwise e.g. Burmese taungban - wing -> P.A. /tãubãː/.
- Vowels and diphthongs with high (breathy) tone become lengthened and followed by a voiced obstruent, usually /z/ e.g. Burmese du:gaung: - knee -> P.A. /duːzgãːuz/.
- Vowels and diphthongs with the creaky tone become are lengthened. Word finally, a voiceless obstruent is inserted e.g. Burmese thi.shi. - know -> P.A. /sʲiːçiːc/ - wise
- Vowels and diphthongs with the checked "tone" are pronounced short, and with whatever consonant is written in the Burmese orthography e.g. Burmese amelaik - hunt, P.A. /ʔameɫaik/. In the case of a monophthong in the last syllable of the word, the consonant is geminated e.g. Burmese naywet - ear -> P.A. /naiwetː/.
- Burmese consonants are palatalised before /i/, and depalatalised before /e/ e.g. Burmese myetsi. - eye -> P.A. /metsʲiːt/.
- Burmese /ɔ/ centralised to schwa e.g. Burmese thittaw: - forest -> P.A. /sʲitːəːz/.
- If a Burmese minor syllable both begins with an obstruent and is followed by an obstruent, the vowel in the minor syllable is deleted (as long as it would not violate Proto-Antarctican phonotactics), causing a consonant cluster e.g. Burmese thei na pati. - general -> P.A. /seinaptʲiːt/
- Burmese initial aspirated consonants become clusters of consonant + /x/ if non-palatalised e.g. Burmese hpoun - dust -> P.A. /pxoːũ/, Burmese hsabin - hair (of the human head) -> P.A. /sxabʲĩ/. If they are palatalised, they become clusters with /ç/ instead e.g. Burmese hpyaun - straight -> P.A. /pʲçãːu/, Burmese yanhpyit - to fight -> P.A. /jãpʲçitː/ - battle.
- Burmese /l/ becomes /ɫ/, unless before /i/ or /j/, when it becomes /ʎ/.
- The alveo-palatal series of consonants become palatal e.g. Burmese yaukkya - man -> P.A. /jaucaː/ (remember Proto-Antarctican does not like geminated consonants after diphthongs).
- Burmese voiceless hl became /sʲ/ e.g. Burmese hle: - to lie down -> P.A. /sʲaːz/.
- Voiceless nasals become prestopped nasals e.g. Burmese hman - correct -> P.A. /pman/.
- Burmese /h/ became /x/.
- Burmese unaspirated /t/ and voiced /d/ became /ts/ and /z/ before /u/.
Thai
A considerable number of Thai words were also borrowed into Antarctican. They were "Antarcticanised" by the following processes:
Vowels
- Thai /i/ palatalised any preceding consonants, or turned a glottal stop into /j/ e.g. Thai /səbaːidiː/ - good health / feeling -> P.A. /sabaːidʲiː/ - "all well".
- Thai /æ/ merged to /a/, but palatalised the preceding consonant in the exact same way e.g. Thai /bæŋ/ - separate -> P.A. /bʲã/.
- Thai /ɨ/ merged into /u/, but palatalised the preceding consonant in the exact same way e.g. Thai /sʲɨːsat/ - faithful -> P.A. /sʲuːsad/.
- Thai /ə/ merged with /a/ e.g. Thai /baŋʔəːn/ - by chance -> P.A. /bãʔãː/ - to stumble upon.
- Thai /ɔː/ centralised to /əː/ e.g. Thai /luːksɔːn/ - arrow, P.A. -> /ɫuːksəː~/
- Thai /ɔːi/ was raised and shortened to /oi/ e.g. Thai /ʔərɔːi/ - tasty -> P.A. /ʔazoi/.
- Thai /əːi/ became /ei/ e.g. Thai /kətʰəːi/ - transsexual -> P.A. /katxei/.
- Thai /æːu/ became /ʲaːu/ e.g. Thai /mæːu/ - cat -> P.A. /mʲaːu/
- Thai /ia/ split into two syllables, becoming /ija/ e.g. Thai /siahaːi/ - destroyed -> P.A. /sʲijaxaːi/
- Thai /ua/ similarly split into two syllables, becoming /uwa/.
- Thai /ɨa/ split into two syllables, becoming /uɥa/ e.g. Thai /rɨa/ - boat -> P.A. /zuɥa/.
- Thai /iau/ split into two syllables, becoming /ijau/ e.g. Thai /tʰiau/ - go out / go on holiday -> P.A. /tʲçijau/.
- Thai /uai/ split into two syllables, becoming /uwai/.
- Thai /ɨai/ split into two syllables, becoming /uɥai/ e.g. Thai /pɨai/ - falling apart -> P.A. /puɥai/.
Consonants
- Final stops became voiced in syllables with low tone e.g. Thai /sàːk/ - pestle -> P.A. /saːg/. Otherwise they became voiceless e.g. Thai /sâːk/ - carcass -> P.A. /saːk/.
- Word final stops were geminated after short vowels e.g. Thai /ʔaːtʰít/ - week -> P.A. /ʔaːtʲçit/.
- Final /m/ had an epenthetic /u/ added after it e.g. Thai /jɔːmpʰæː/ - give up -> P.A. /jəːmupʲçaː/ - surrender.
- Other nasals disappeared at the end of syllables, nasalising the preceding vowel e.g. Thai /ʔajakaːn/ - prosecutor -> P.A. /ʔajakãː/ - to prosecute.
- The evolution of affricates is unpredictable. Sometimes they became palatal, sometimes palatalised alveolar. The aspirated affricate either became a fricative or a voiceless stop. The unaspirated affricate either became a voiced or a voiceless stop.
- Apart from this, aspirated consonants behaved like in words from Burmese. They tended to become clusters of consonant + /x/ (or /ç if before /i/ or /j/) e.g. Thai /tʰai/ -> P.A. /txai/, Thai /kʰəjan/ - hard-working -> P.A. /cçã/.
- Outside of clusters, Thai /r/ fricativised to /z/ e.g. e.g. Thai /ʔərɔːi/ - tasty -> P.A. /ʔazoi/.
- In a cluster, Thai /r/ became /aɫ/ e.g. Thai /kròːt/ - angry, P.A. /kaɫoːd/. However, before /i/, /ɨ/ and /æ/, it became /ʎ/ instead e.g. Thai - /priau/ - sour -> P.A. /paʎijau/.
- Outside a cluster, Thai /l/ became /ɫ/ e.g. Thai /leːu/ - bad, naughty -> P.A. /ɫeːu/. However, before /i/, /ɨ/ or /æ/, it became /ʎ/ e.g. Thai /kʰiːlɨːm/ - forgetful -> P.A. /cçiːʎuːmu/ - absent-minded.
- In a cluster, Thai /l/ became /eɫ/ normally e.g. Thai /pluːk/ - to plant -> P.A. /peluːg/. However, it became /eʎ/ before /i/ and /æ/ e.g. Thai - /plianplæːŋ/ - to change -> P.A. /peʎijãpeʎãː/.
- In a cluster, Thai /w/ became /aw/ e.g. Thai /kʰwaːi/ - water buffalo -> P.A. /kxawaːi/.
- /ŋ/ became /ŋg/ and then /g/ before a vowel, and disappeared with nasalisation of the preceding vowel otherwise e.g. Thai /ŋoŋŋuai/ - astonished -> P.A. /gõguwai/.
- While voiceless nasals are no longer a feature of spoken Thai (only being found in the written language), they were revived via spelling pronunciations and became prestopped nasals in Proto-Antarctican e.g. Thai /maːk/ - betel nut -> P.A. /pmaːg/.
- Thai /h/ became /x/.
- Thai unaspirated /t/ and voiced /d/ became /ts/ and /z/ before /u/.
Indonesian/Malay/Tagalog
Given the restrictive nature of the phonology of these languages, words borrowed from these languages did not need to go undergo many sound changes in order to become Antacticanised. The most significant changes were the fricativisation of /r/ to /z/, the insertion of /j/ and palatalisation of consonants before /i/, the loss of /j/ and depalatalisation of consonants before /e/, and the loss of syllable final /n/ and /ŋ/, with nasalisation on the preceding vowel.
Also, vowels in Tagalog with irregular stress were borrowed in as long vowels, otherwise they were borrowed in as short vowels.
And since Proto-Antarctican did not permit alveolar stops before /u/, in this situation, /t/ and /d/ became /ts/ and /z/ respectively.
Vietnamese
Apart from general "Antarctification" of loanwords e.g. palatalisation before /i/, turning aspirate stops into clusters with a fricative, the change of /h/ into /x/ etc., there are a few changes that applied especially to Vietnamese words.
Vowels
- As a rule, vowels were adapted into the language in the same way as Thai.
- The vowels a and o' always became long /a/.
- The vowels ă and â always became short /a/.
- The length of the rest of the vowels depended on the tone of the word. In the case of the ngang and huyền tones, they were lengthened. Otherwise, they were shortened.
- Vowels with the ngã tone were split into two syllables separated by a glottal stop e.g. Vietnamese nhẵn - smooth -> P.A. /nʲaʔã/.
Consonants
- Word final consonants were pronounced voiced in words with the nặng tone, and voiceless in words with the sắc tone.
- Vietnamese ng(h) and g(h) are both pronounced /g/ before a vowel.
- There is a lot of inconsistency in consonants in borrowed words. This is due to the fact that some words were borrowed from Hanoi Vietnamese, while others were borrowed from Ho Chi Minh Vietnamese.
- Aspirated consonants generally behaved like those from Burmese.
- Vietnamese unaspirated /t/ and voiced /d/ became /ts/ and /z/ before /u/.
Taiwanese
More of the refugees who came to Antarctica were from Taiwan rather than the mainland. Therefore, Taiwanese has influenced Proto-Antarctican far more than Mandarin. Aspirated consonants generally behaved like those from Burmese. Syllable final stops were pronounced voiced in words with tone 4, and voiceless in words with tone 8. Vowels in words with tones 1, 2, 5 and 6 were pronounced long, otherwise they were short. Taiwanese unaspirated /t/ and voiced /d/ became /ts/ and /z/ before /u/.
Evolution into Antarctican
Proto-Antarctican further evolved into Antarctican, with several sound changes having a large effect on the language. These sound changes happened in a number of stages:
Development of Velar and Palatal Nasals
- If a glottal stop was preceded by a nasal vowel, it changed to a prestopped velar nasal /kŋ/, with denasalisation of the preceding vowel e.g. /wãʔaːtʲçit/ - Sunday -> /wakŋaːtʲçit/.
- If /g/ was preceded by an oral vowel, and followed by another vowel, then it changed to /ŋ/ e.g. /doʎaːgõ/ - dragon -> /doʎaːŋõ/.
- The palatalised alveolar nasals (both plain and prestopped) became palatal nasals.
Origin of Glottal Codas
- Clusters which ended in a non-alveolar fricative were broken up by inserting an epenthetic /a/ e.g. /pxõːu/ - dust -> /paxõːu/, or /ʔaːtʲçit/ - week -> /ʔaːtʲaçit/
- Voiceless consonants that were not followed by a vowel became a glottal stop e.g. /seinaptʲiːt/ - general -> /seinaʔtʲiːʔ/. Note that this did not affect prestopped nasals, since they counted as a single consonant.
- Voiceless geminate consonants became sequences of glottal stop + consonant e.g. /kucːik/ - a female given name -> /kuʔciʔ/ (the above sound change also applies, changing the final /k/ into a glottal stop.
- Voiced consonants that were not followed by a vowel became a glottal fricative /ɦ/ e.g. /zbõ/ - pants -> /ɦbõ/, or /saːg/ - pestle -> /saːɦ/
- Voiced geminate consonants became sequences of /ɦ/ + consonant e.g. /ɫedːo/ - red -> /ɫeɦdo/.
Tonogenesis
The next step was for /ʔ/ and /ɦ/ to influence the pitch and phonation of the preceding vowels.
Breathy Phonation
- /ɦ/ put breathy voice on the vowel that preceded it, and lowered the pitch e.g.
/kaːɦ/ - membership card (the final /d/ had debuccalised to /ɦ/) -> /kàːɦ/
- This breathy voice would spread through voiced consonants to affect multiple syllables e.g. /ʔunaɦ/ - eel (the final /ɟ had debuccalised to /ɦ/) -> /ʔùnàɦ/.
- If the breathy voice would "spread" through an initial consonant, then that word acquired floating phonation e.g. /ɫeɦdo/ - red -> /`ɫèɦdo/.
- Word initial clusters beginning with /ɦ/ also created floating phonation e.g. /ɦbõ/ - pants (the initial /z/ had debuccalised to /ɦ/) -> /`ɦbõ/.
Tense Phonation
- Similarly, the glottal stop put tense voice on the vowel and raised the pitch e.g. /webːoʔ/ (the final /s/ had debuccalised to a glottal stop) - eggs -> /webːóʔ/.
- In some cases, this tense voice spread through consonants and affect multiple vowels. Consonants that permitted this spreading were voiced sonorants and voiceless stops (other than the glottal stop) e.g. /ʔameɫaiʔ/ - hunt (the final /k/ had debuccalised to a glottal stop) -> /ʔáméɫáiʔ/.
- If the tense voice would "spread" through an initial consonant, then that word acquired floating phonation e.g. /tẽʔ/ - weather (the final /c/ had debuccalised to a glottal stop) -> /'té~ʔ/.
- Word initial clusters beginning with glottal stops also created floating phonation e.g. /'ʔcidáʔ/ - drawers (from /çcidasʲ/).
Vowel Changes
After vowel phonation became phonemic, a series of major changes occurred to the vowels of Proto-Antarctican:
Chain Shifts
While Antarctican has a great deal more vowel phonemes than Proto-Antarctican, the first vowel shifts to occur actually took the language in the other direction, causing the loss of /o/. What happened depended on whether or not the vowel was preceded by /j/ or a palatalised consonant. These two sound changes below are the reason why vowels in Antarctican change if their preceding consonant changes from soft to hard or vice versa.
After /j/ or a Palatalised Consonant
This chain shift began with the fronting of /a/ to /e/ after /j/ or a palatalised consonant e.g. /'máʎájáːʔ/ - manners -> /'máʎéjéːʔ/ (notice how the first /a/ was unaffected, since it was preceded by /m/).
In the same situation, /jo/ and /ʲa/ lowered to /jo/ and /ʲa/ respectively e.g. /'tʲókóɫéːʔ/ - chocolate -> /'tʲákóɫéːʔ/.
Also, the vowel /iu/ became /uː/ (though the palatalisation of the preceding consonant was retained) e.g. /paxaʔcaçiːʎiu/ - tripe -> /paxáʔceçiːʎu#720;/ (notice how /ca/ changed to /ce/ as well).
After a Non-Palatalised Consonant
After this, a second, anti-clockwise chain shift began with the fronting of short /u/ to /i/ e.g. /zuɥa/ - boat -> /ziɥe/ (the /a/ shifted to /e/ because of the preceding sound change). The diphthong /ui/ became long /iː/ e.g. /pmui/ - cold (weather) -> /pmiː/ (note that these never created any new homophones because previously /i/ had only occurred before /j/ or a palatalised cononant.). Short /o/ was then raised to /u/ e.g. /doko/ - where -> /duku/.
Finally, /oi/ and /oːi/ shifted to /ui/ and /uːi/ respectively e.g. /kamoːi/ - thief -> /kamuːi/, or /ʔazoi/ - tasty -> /ʔazui/.
Vowel Splits
The previous two chain shifts left the vowel system profoundly unbalanced, with two front vowels /e/ and /i/, two central vowels /a/ and /ə/ (the latter only long), but only one back vowel /u/. It was not long before this was corrected by another vowel shift, which also dramatically increased the number of vowel phonemes in Antarctican.
This split depended on the phonation of the syllable containing the vowel, and also in some cases the phonation of the following syllable. Tense voiced vowels were lowered and/or fronted (if they were high, they also diphthongised). Breathy voiced vowels were raised and/or backed. If both the vowel and the one in the syllable after it had modal voice, the first vowel was also shifted depending on the voicing of the intervening consonant. Voiced consonants lowered the vowel, voiceless ones raised and/or centred it. The glottal stop had no effect.
Original Vowel | Tense Voiced Vowel | Breathy Voiced Vowel | Modally Voiced Vowel | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Following Syllable has Voiced Consonant Followed by Modally Voiced Vowel | Following Syllable has Voiceless Consonant Followed by Modally Voiced Vowel | Other Cases | |||
i | ei | ɨ | i | ɨ | i |
iː | ɘi | ɨi | iː | ɨː | iː |
e(ː) | ɛ(ː) | ɘ(ː) | e(ː) | ɘ(ː) | e(ː) |
e(ː)i | ɛ(ː)i | ɘ(ː)i | e(ː)i | ɘ(ː)i | e(ː)i |
e(ː)u | ɛ(ː)u | ɘ(ː)u | e(ː)u | ɘ(ː)u | e(ː)u |
a(ː) | a(ː) | ɜ(ː) | a(ː) | ɛ(ː) | a(ː) |
a(ː)i | a(ː)i | ɜ(ː)i | a(ː)i | ɛ(ː)i | a(ː)i |
a(ː)u | a(ː)u | ɜ(ː)u | a(ː)u | ɛ(ː)u | a(ː)u |
u | ou | u | o | u | u |
uː | eu | ɨu | ou | uː | uː |
u(ː)i | o(ː)i | u(ː)i | o(ː)i | u(ː)i | u(ː)i |
o(ː)u | ɔ(ː)u | o(ː)u | ɔ(ː)u | o(ː)u | o(ː)u |
əː | ɜː | o | ɒ | ɔ | ɔ |
This is the source of the vowel mutations that happen in Antarctican when a vowel changes its phonation.
Other Consonant Changes
Ejectives
Clusters of glottal stop + obstruent became ejectives before a vowel with tense voice e.g.
/'ʔkóuʔ/ - a little bit -> /kʼouʔ/.
/ʔóuʔfʲéiʔ/ - office -> /ʔóufʼʲéiʔ/
Laterals
The velarised alveolar lateral /ɫ/ lost its laterality and became uvular /ʁ/. The palatal lateral /ʎ/ became alveolar /l/ (which still has /ʎ/ as an allophone before high vowels).
Antarctican also acquired lateral obstruents /tɬ/, /dɮ/, /ɬ/ from the palatalised alveolar consonants /tʲ/, /dʲ/ and /sʲ/ respectively. This was via an intermediate sound change to clusters of /tʎ/, /dʎ/ and /ʎ/ The voiced palatalised fricative /zʲ/ was lost, merging into /dʲ/, which then became /dʎ/ and finally /dɮ/.
Loss of Voicing Contrasts
After this, phonemic voicing was lost in most places in the language. The only consonants for which it was retained were either:
- Those between two instances of modal phonation (vowels or floating phonation), or:
- Those between two instances of breathy phonation (vowels or floating phonation).
Prestopped Nasals
Except for these two cases, prestopped nasals became plain e.g. /ʔɛtetnɜɦluɴɦ/ - heart -> /ʔɛtenɜɦluɴɦ/
Stops / Affricates
Stops and affricates usually became voiceless e.g. /dámá/ - victim of a scam -> /támá/.
The only exception were those preceded by breathy phonation, and followed by another kind of phonation, which became voiced e.g. /`mʲɨbõ/ - my pants, which underwent no change.
Stops and affricates between two instances of breathy phonation usually became voiceless e.g. /`mʲɨ`bɨ`lɨ`ɦ/ - my bridge -> /`mʲɨ`pɨ`lɨ`ɦ/
The exception was those clustered with /ɦ/, which became voiced e.g. /`mʲɨ`ɦbʲɨ`ɦ/.
Sibilants
/s/ and /z/ patterned with stops and affricates here e.g. /`zɜ`;mɨ`ɦ/ - (major wife) -> /`sɜ`mɨ`ɦ/
Spirant Lenition
However, in similar circumstances, non-sibilant fricatives (i.e. everything other than /s/ and /z/) became approximants e.g. /teiɬɛ'ːʔ/ - shirt -> /teilɛ'ːʔ/.
This sound change did not apply to ejective fricatives, which (apart from /sʼ/), became plain voiceless fricatives e.g. /ʔóufʼʲéiʔ/ - office - /ʔóufʲéiʔ/
Glottal Codas
Finally, /ɦ/ was lost everywhere, and /ʔ/ was lost everywhere before a vowel.
Case Marking
Ergative Case
The ergative case suffix /ga/ underwent some developments. After a modally voiced back vowel, it lowered the vowel (all voiced consonants did this to a preceding back vowel as long as they were followed by a modally voiced vowel) and then disappeared. If this sound change could not apply, but the preceding vowel was oral, then it became nasal. This was done via the suffix changing to /ŋa/ as an intermediary.
Comitative Case
As similar development happened to the comitative case marker /mo/ -> /pmu/. After a modally voiced non-back vowel, it shifted the vowel and then vanished. Otherwise it remained as /pmu/ or /ʔu/.
Sample Vocabulary
Below is a list of words in Proto-Antarctican, along with their etymology and their counterparts in Antarctican:
Source Language | Word in Source Language | Meaning in Source Language | Proto-Antarctican Word | Antarctican Word | Meaning in Antarctican |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
English | mile | mile | moju | muju | mile |
English | vagabond | vagabond | ɥagːabõd | ʱɥɘʱgɜʱpuʱ | vagabond |
English | book | book | bukː | peiˤ | book |
English | office | office | ʔofːʲis | ʔouˤfʲeiˤ | office |
English | brother | brother | buɫaza | biʁaza | brother |
Japanese | youfuku | clothes | joːfku | ˤjaːˤki | clothes |
Japanese | zubon | pants | zbõ | ʱboɴ | pants |
Japanese | sakki | some time ago | sacː | saˤ | some time ago |
Japanese | mazu | firstly | maz | ʱmɜʱ | firstly |
Japanese | shiboru | to squeeze | sʲibou | ɬibou | squeeze |
Japanese | kanjiru | to feel | kãdʲiu | kaɴdɮuː | to feel |
Japanese | kazoeru | to count | kazoweu | kazoweu | to count |
Japanese | nagareru | to flow | nagaɫeu | naŋaʁeu | to flow |
Japanese | naoru | to get better | najou | najou | to get better |
Spanish | mañana | morning | manʲana | maɲana | morning |
Spanish | cabeza | head | kabesːa | kapɛˤsa | head |
Spanish | pensar | to think | pẽsaː | pɘɴsaː | lost in deep thought |
Spanish | tierra | land | teza | teza | landscape |
Spanish | lima | Lima (in Peru) | ʎima | lima | Lima (in Peru) |
Spanish | palo | stick | paɫo | paʁu | stick |
Spanish | derecha | right (as in not left) | deɫetːʲa | tɛˤʁɛˤtɬe | right (as in not left) |
Spanish | golpear | to hit | goupejaː | goupejeː | to suffer a setback |
Spanish | huevos | eggs | webːos | ʱwɘʱpouˤ | egg |
Spanish | juego | game | fegːo | wɘʱgu | game |
Spanish | fuego | fire | fuwegːo | wɨʱwɘʱgu | fire |
Spanish | viuda | widow | bʲudːa | pʲuʱda | widow |
Burmese | taungban | wing | tãubãː | tauɴbaː | wing |
Burmese | du:gaung: | knee | duːzgãːuz | ʱtɨuʱgɜːu | knee |
Burmese | thi.shi. | to know | sʲiːçiːc | ɬiːjɘiˤ | wise |
Burmese | amelaik | to hunt | ʔameɫaik | ʔaˤmɛˤʁaiˤ | prey |
Burmese | naywet | ear | naiwetː | ˤnaiˤwɛˤ | ear |
Burmese | myetsi | eye | metsʲiːt | ˤmɛˤɬɘiˤ | eye |
Burmese | thittaw: | forest | sʲitːəːz | leiˤtoʱ | forest |
Burmese | tei na pati. | general | seinaptʲiːt | sɛiˤnaˤtʼɬɘiˤ | general |
Burmese | hpoun | dust | pxoːũ | pɛχoːuɴ | dust |
Burmese | hsabin | hair (on the head) | sxabʲĩ | saxabʲiɴ | hair (on the head) |
Burmese | hypaun | straight | pʲçãːu | pʲɘçaːuɴ | straight |
Burmese | yanhpyit | to fight | jãpʲçitː | jɘɴpʲejeiˤ | battle |
Burmese | yaukkya | man | jaucaː | jauceː | man |
Burmese | hle: | to lie down | sʲaːz | lɘːʱ | to lie down |
Burmese | hman | correct | pmã | pmaɴ | correct |
Thai | /sʲɨːsat/ | faithful | sʲuːsad | ɬuːsɜʱ | faithful |
Thai | /səbaːidiː/ | good health / feeling | sabaːidʲiː | sabaːidɮiː | all well |
Thai | /bæŋ/ | separate | bʲã | bʲaɴ | separate |
Thai | /baŋʔəːn/ | by chance | bãʔãː | bɛkŋaːɴ | to stumble upon |
Thai | /ʔərɔːi/ | tasty | ʔazoi | ʔazui | tasty |
Thai | /kətʰəːi/ | transsexual | katxei | kɛtɛxei | transsexual |
Thai | /siahaːi/ | destroyed | sʲijaxaːi | ɬijɛxaːi | to destroy |
Thai | /rɨa/ | boat | zuɥa | ziɥe | boat |
Thai | /mæːu/ | cat | mʲaːu | mʲeːu | cat |
Thai | /tʰiau/ | go out / go on holiday | tʲçijau | tɬɛçijeu | go out / go on holiday |
Thai | /pɨai/ | falling apart | puɥai | piɥei | to fall apart |
Thai | /sàːk/ | pestle | saːg | sɜːʱ | pestle |
Thai | /sâːk/ | carcass | saːk | saːˤ | carcass |
Thai | /ʔaːtʰít/ | week | ʔaːtʲçit | ʔɛːtɬejeiˤ | week |
Thai | /wanʔaːtʰít/ | Sunday | wãʔaːtʲçit | wɛkŋɛːtɬejeiˤ | Sunday |
Thai | /jɔːmpʰæː/ | give up | jəːmupʲçaː | jɔmɨpʲɘçeː | surrender |
Thai | /ʔajakaːn/ | prosecutor | ʔajakãː | ʔajɘkaːɴ | to prosecute |
Thai | /kʰəjan/ | hard-working | cçã | cɘçaɴ | hard-working |
Thai | /kròːt/ | angry | kaɫoːd | kɜʱʁuːʱ | angry |
Thai | /priau/ | sour | paʎijau | palijeu | sour |
Thai | /kʰiːlɨːm/ | forgetful | cçiːʎuːmu | cɘçiːloːmi | absent-minded |
Thai | /leːu/ | bad, naughty | ɫeːu | ʁeːu | naughty |
Thai | /plianplæːŋ/ | to change | peʎijãpeʎãː | pelijɛɴpeleːɴ | to change |
Thai | /kʰwaːi/ | water buffalo | kxawaːi | kɛxawaːi | water buffalo |
Thai | /ŋoŋŋuai/ | astonished | gõguwai | goɴgiwai | astonished |
Thai | /maːk/ | betel nut | pmaːg | mɜːʱ | betel nut |
Vietnamese | nhẵn | smooth | nʲaʔã | ɲeʔaɴ | smooth |