45,638
edits
Chrysophylax (talk | contribs) m (categoramising) |
No edit summary |
||
(5 intermediate revisions by 2 users not shown) | |||
Line 2: | Line 2: | ||
|name = Sprik | |name = Sprik | ||
|nativename = Sprik | |nativename = Sprik | ||
|pronunciation = | |pronunciation = sprɪk | ||
| | |setting = six towns in Moselle, France | ||
|speakers = < 200 | |speakers = < 200 | ||
|date = 2012 | |date = 2012 | ||
|familycolor = Indo-European | |familycolor = Indo-European | ||
|fam2 = [[w:Germanic_languages|Germanic]] | |fam2 = [[w:Germanic_languages|Germanic]] | ||
|fam3 = [[w:West_Germanic_languages|West Germanic]] | |fam3 = [[w:West_Germanic_languages|West Germanic]] | ||
|fam4 = [[w: | |fam4 = [[w:Low Franconian languages|Low Franconian]] | ||
| | |script1 = Latn | ||
|creator = User:Zev | |||
| | |||
}} | }} | ||
Line 46: | Line 38: | ||
;Prefixed verbs. | ;Prefixed verbs. | ||
Verbs have two kinds of prefixes, separable and inseparable. Inseparable prefixes, like ''un-'', simply become part of the verbal stem and do not change what case the verb takes if it is transitive (the default is the accusative). Separable prefixes are prepositions in their own right, and the verb once thus prefixed will take whatever case the preposition does (usually the dative). The most common such prefix is ''fer-''', which as an independent preposition means “for” but as a prefix renders an intransitive verb transitive; we shall use ''fersprikin'' (“to tell”) as an example. The prefix is removed from the front and placed after the verb in all the conjugated forms to produce ''mij sprek fer hooi'' (“I tell him”) and ''et sprich fer hooi'' (“it tells him”). The past participle places ''ge-'' between the prefix and the stem, as in ''fergesprikt''. If one wanted to say “he speaks for him”, it would be necessary to put the object in the genitive (and if it is a pronoun, to substitute the appropriate possessive adjective for the nonexistent genitive) as ''hij spricht fer hijn''. | Verbs have two kinds of prefixes, separable and inseparable. Inseparable prefixes, like ''un-'', simply become part of the verbal stem and do not change what case the verb takes if it is transitive (the default is the accusative). Separable prefixes are prepositions in their own right, and the verb once thus prefixed will take whatever case the preposition does (usually the dative). The most common such prefix is ''fer-''', which as an independent preposition means “for” but as a prefix renders an intransitive verb transitive; we shall use ''fersprikin'' (“to tell”) as an example. The prefix is removed from the front and placed after the verb in all the conjugated forms to produce ''mij sprek fer hooi'' (“I tell him”) and ''et sprich fer hooi'' (“it tells him”). The past participle places ''ge-'' between the prefix and the stem, as in ''fergesprikt''. If one wanted to say “he speaks for him”, it would be necessary to put the object in the genitive (and if it is a pronoun, to substitute the appropriate possessive adjective for the nonexistent genitive) as ''hij spricht fer hijn''. A bitransitive verb will normally take the second object in the dative case or use an intervening preposition to make the meaning clearer, but bitransitive verbs that take the dative case (mainly verbs with separable prefixes) take the second object in the genitive case. | ||
;Irregular verbs. | ;Irregular verbs. | ||
Line 52: | Line 44: | ||
;Articles and numerals. | ;Articles and numerals. | ||
The definite article is ''dij'', ''dei'', ''den'', ''dei''. The indefinite article is ''ij'', ''ei'', ''ei'', ''ei''. If the next word begins with a vowel, or if the article is the last word of a sentence or standalone statement, ''-n'' will be added to the indefinite article to produce ''ijn'', ''ein'', ''ein'', ''ein''. Many speakers will not decline the articles in rapid or informal speech, instead using the forms ''de'' and ''e(n)'' (rarely written, so the spelling may vary). The indefinite article is also used for the number one, in which case it is always fully pronounced and declined. No other numbers decline. The cardinal numbers one to twenty are ''ij''(''n''), ''tvei'', ''trie'', ''fer'', ''fief'', ''zij'', ''zefin'', ''ijcht'', ''nefin'', ''tijn'', ''ijten'', ''tveten'', ''treten'', ''ferten'', ''fiften'', ''zijten'', ''zeften'', ''ijchten'', ''neften'', ''tveintik''. In the towns, where the digraph ''ch'' is pronounced more softly, it is common to hear ''ijkten'' for eighteen to avoid confusion with ''ijten''. There is no word for zero, but ''nijn'' (“none”) is commonly accepted in such use. The ordinals are regularly declining adjectives. | The definite article is ''dij'', ''dei'', ''den'', ''dei''. The indefinite article is ''ij'', ''ei'', ''ei'', ''ei''. If the next word begins with a vowel, or if the article is the last word of a sentence or standalone statement, ''-n'' will be added to the indefinite article to produce ''ijn'', ''ein'', ''ein'', ''ein''. Many speakers will not decline the articles in rapid or informal speech, instead using the forms ''de'' and ''e(n)'' (rarely written, so the spelling may vary). The indefinite article is also used for the number one, in which case it is always fully pronounced and declined. No other numbers decline. The cardinal numbers one to twenty are ''ij''(''n''), ''tvei'', ''trie'', ''fer'', ''fief'', ''zij'', ''zefin'', ''ijcht'', ''nefin'', ''tijn'', ''ijten'', ''tveten'', ''treten'', ''ferten'', ''fiften'', ''zijten'', ''zeften'', ''ijchten'', ''neften'', ''tveintik''. In the towns, where the digraph ''ch'' is pronounced more softly, it is common to hear ''ijkten'' for eighteen to avoid confusion with ''ijten''. There is no word for zero, but ''nijn'' (“none”) is commonly accepted in such use. The ordinals are regularly declining adjectives, and are used to form fractions as well (so "one half" is ''ij tveidt''). | ||
;Borrowings. | ;Borrowings. | ||
Line 62: | Line 54: | ||
===Addendum: Phonology=== | ===Addendum: Phonology=== | ||
Sprik has no agreed-upon phonology, in part because of the lack of a standard prestige dialect, and in part due to lack of study. Recordings have only been made of male speakers in the northwest sector, specifically from the town of Mont St. Gerard, and are reported to resemble “if you got a Dutch guy, and an Australian guy, and tried to make them compete to see whose vowels could overpower the others” ( | Sprik has no agreed-upon phonology, in part because of the lack of a standard prestige dialect, and in part due to lack of study. Recordings have only been made of male speakers in the northwest sector, specifically from the town of Mont St. Gerard, and are reported to resemble “if you got a Dutch guy, and an Australian guy, and tried to make them compete to see whose vowels could overpower the others” (Meredith 2013). | ||
This following analysis is the most complete to date, and yet important features like glottal stops that occur regularly in native speakers' speech are still completely ignored. It is simply given as a general guide. Below is a table that shows correspondences between the orthography and the phonology in IPA, although one should remember that although <a> and <r> are represented in this analysis as [a] and [r], they are more likely closer to [ɑ] and [ɻ] to many speakers. This analysis is biased toward the highly incomplete and imperfect studies done to date, and is not representative of all speakers. | |||
;Orthography | {| {{Table/bluetable}} style="text-align:center; vertical-align:middle" | ||
! style="text-align: center;"|'''Orthography''' | |||
! style="text-align: center;"|'''Phonology''' | |||
! style="text-align: center;"|'''Notes''' | |||
|- | |||
|a | |||
|[a] | |||
|[ɑː] when doubled | |||
|- | |||
|b | |||
|[b] | |||
|[p] at ends of words | |||
|- | |||
|ch | |||
|[ç] | |||
|often [h] when preceded by a vowel and followed by a consonant | |||
|- | |||
|d | |||
|[d] | |||
|[t] at ends of words | |||
|- | |||
|e | |||
|[ɛ] | |||
|[ə] when unstressed or at the ends of words | |||
|- | |||
|ei | |||
|[aɪ̯] | |||
| | |||
|- | |||
|f | |||
|[f] | |||
| | |||
|- | |||
|g | |||
|[g] | |||
|[k] at ends of words | |||
|- | |||
|h | |||
|[h] | |||
|[x] on occasion; see below | |||
|- | |||
|i | |||
|[ɪ] | |||
|[ə] when unstressed or at the ends of words | |||
|- | |||
|ie | |||
|[i] | |||
|[iː] in most positions | |||
|- | |||
|j | |||
|[j] | |||
| | |||
|- | |||
|ij | |||
|[əi̯] | |||
|considered a single letter | |||
|- | |||
|k | |||
|[k] | |||
| | |||
|- | |||
|l | |||
|[l] | |||
| | |||
|- | |||
|m | |||
|[m] | |||
| | |||
|- | |||
|n | |||
|[n] | |||
|[ŋ] before [g] or [k] | |||
|- | |||
|o | |||
|[ɔ] | |||
|[oə] when doubled | |||
|- | |||
|p | |||
|[p] | |||
| | |||
|- | |||
|r | |||
|[r] | |||
|[ʁ], [ɚ] or absent at the ends of syllables | |||
|- | |||
|s | |||
|[s] | |||
| | |||
|- | |||
|t | |||
|[t] | |||
| | |||
|- | |||
|u | |||
|[ø] | |||
| | |||
|- | |||
|v | |||
|[ʋ] | |||
|[v] to many speakers | |||
|- | |||
|y | |||
|[ə] | |||
|variable realisation, often around [ɵ] | |||
|- | |||
|z | |||
|[z] | |||
| | |||
|- | |||
|} | |||
Final voiced consonants are regularly devoiced, with the exception of /z/. The phoneme /h/ is often pronounced as [x] at the beginning of a sentence, and in a few cases it does seem to be phonemic (as noted above, in differentiating ''hooi'' ("hello") and ''hooi'' ("to him")). It should be noted that *''dtt'' is an illegal consonant cluster, and is resolved as ''dt'' where it may be predicted in a verb's conjugational pattern. Similarly, an adjective or noun that ends in ''-dt'' in its lemma form would inflect by deleting the ''t'' before adding an inflectional suffix. | |||
Stress is irregular, but the default stress is penultimate. For verbs, the past participle takes ultimate stress. For adjectives and nouns, inflectional suffixes do not move the stress from its position in the lemma form. Borrowings are usually stressed as they were in the language they are being borrowed from, although when Latinate words are borrowed, if the Sprik form has less syllables than the original Latin form, the original stress is usually ignored and penultimate stress is applied. Compounds generally take the stress of the words being compounded, wherein the stress of polysyllabic words overrides the stress of words with less syllables, and compounds where all the elements are monosyllabic take penultimate stress. | |||
===Appendix=== | ===Appendix=== | ||
Line 95: | Line 190: | ||
<!-- Categories --> | <!-- Categories --> | ||
[[Category:Languages]] | [[Category:Languages]] | ||
[[Category: | [[Category:Languages]] | ||
[[Category:West Germanic languages]] | [[Category:West Germanic languages]] |